Skip to main content

An application of upscaled optimal foraging theory using hidden Markov modelling: year-round behavioural variation in a large arctic herbivore

Abstract

Background

In highly seasonal environments, animals face critical decisions regarding time allocation, diet optimisation, and habitat use. In the Arctic, the short summers are crucial for replenishing body reserves, while low food availability and increased energetic demands characterise the long winters (9–10 months). Under such extreme seasonal variability, even small deviations from optimal time allocation can markedly impact individuals’ condition, reproductive success and survival. We investigated which environmental conditions influenced daily, seasonal, and interannual variation in time allocation in high-arctic muskoxen (Ovibos moschatus) and evaluated whether results support qualitative predictions derived from upscaled optimal foraging theory.

Methods

Using hidden Markov models (HMMs), we inferred behavioural states (foraging, resting, relocating) from hourly positions of GPS-collared females tracked in northeast Greenland (28 muskox-years). To relate behavioural variation to environmental conditions, we considered a wide range of spatially and/or temporally explicit covariates in the HMMs.

Results

While we found little interannual variation, daily and seasonal time allocation varied markedly. Scheduling of daily activities was distinct throughout the year except for the period of continuous daylight. During summer, muskoxen spent about 69% of time foraging and 19% resting, without environmental constraints on foraging activity. During winter, time spent foraging decreased to 45%, whereas about 43% of time was spent resting, mediated by longer resting bouts than during summer.

Conclusions

Our results clearly indicate that female muskoxen follow an energy intake maximisation strategy during the arctic summer. During winter, our results were not easily reconcilable with just one dominant foraging strategy. The overall reduction in activity likely reflects higher time requirements for rumination in response to the reduction of forage quality (supporting an energy intake maximisation strategy). However, deep snow and low temperatures were apparent constraints to winter foraging, hence also suggesting attempts to conserve energy (net energy maximisation strategy). Our approach provides new insights into the year-round behavioural strategies of the largest Arctic herbivore and outlines a practical example of how to approximate qualitative predictions of upscaled optimal foraging theory using multi-year GPS tracking data.

Background

To improve reproductive success and survival, wild animals adjust their behaviour and scheduling of activities to varying resource availability, environmental conditions and risk levels [1]. Assessing how animals fine-tune their behaviour to fluctuating conditions is thus fundamental to our understanding of how species cope with ecological constraints. This is especially urgent in the context of climate change, which may alter the range of biotic and abiotic conditions animals encounter. Arctic and alpine herbivores are challenged by some of the greatest seasonal differences in foraging, light and climatic conditions. Consequently, movement behaviour and foraging strategies are expected to vary markedly according to daily, seasonal, and interannual changes in local conditions [2,3,4,5].

Balancing trade-offs between energetic costs and gains, predation risk and environmental constraints, animals are constantly faced with decisions regarding optimal foraging timing, space and diet [6]. Traditionally, optimal foraging theory (OFT) distinguished between energy intake maximisation and time minimising strategies [6, 7]. Energy-maximising animals optimise energy intake by allocating most of their available time to feeding, whereas time-minimisers should only feed for the time necessary to satisfy minimum energetic requirements. While the former strategy yields the greatest amount of energy for maintenance, growth and reproduction, the latter provides the minimum amount of energy required to fulfil basic energetic needs while allowing higher time allocation to behaviours that improve survival, such as staying inactive during certain periods of the day to reduce the risk of predation. For herbivores in extreme environments, constraints such as adverse weather conditions may also be energetically limiting factors, and a net energy maximisation (i.e. energy conservation) strategy has therefore been proposed as additional potential optimal foraging strategy [2, 5, 8, 9]. This strategy is identical to the energy intake maximisation strategy as long as foraging provides a net energy gain. However, when the gained benefits do not outweigh the energetic costs of the foraging effort, animals should attempt to conserve energy instead of maximising intake. As constraints on foraging decisions can vary considerably between seasons, animals may adopt different strategies over the course of the year [5].

Conventionally, studies testing predictions of OFT focus on fine-scale foraging behaviour, usually assessed in experimental settings with direct observational data collected over relatively short periods and at small spatial scales. However, foraging behaviour of free-ranging animals follows a hierarchy of spatial and temporal scales, from bites at the food patch level to larger movements, representing foraging decision-making at daily, seasonal, and annual scales [10, 11]. Assessing optimality based on the maximisation of short-term gains may be misleading when considering longer time scales, and Owen-Smith et al. [12] therefore proposed an ‘upscaled’ approach to OFT, where the basic principles of OFT also apply to larger-scale movement behaviour. This approach is especially applicable to terrestrial herbivores, as vegetation usually remains constant in its spatial distribution, but may vary substantially in both availability and quality over time [12]. Indeed, interpreting activity budgets and behavioural responses to environmental conditions against qualitative predictions derived from (upscaled) OFT has proven useful to approximate seasonal behavioural strategies and to identify key constraints on foraging [2, 8, 13].

Thus far, studying the behaviours of wild animals and drivers thereof has been difficult, especially in remote regions and over time periods covering seasonal as well as interannual variability in environmental conditions. However, improvements in tracking technologies now allow recordings of high-precision animal movements over extended periods, independent of weather and light conditions even in inaccessible regions, while greatly reducing sampling or observer bias [14]. Simultaneously with these technological advancements, behaviour-focused modelling approaches have evolved, designed to detect different behaviours from telemetry data and to investigate their relationship with environmental conditions [15]. In particular, hidden Markov models (HMMs) have emerged as flexible behaviour-based tools for the analysis of regular observational time series driven by underlying, serially correlated states [16]. Moreover, physics-based numerical models explicit in space and through time (e.g. MicroMet and SnowModel [17, 18]) are increasingly capable of providing realistic environmental data at resolutions and extents relevant for ecological applications [19, 20], i.e. data traditionally unavailable from in situ measurements. Combined, these developments in data acquisition and analyses are greatly improving our ability to assess wildlife-environment interactions.

The muskox (Ovibos moschatus) is the largest arctic herbivore, well adapted to a cold and highly seasonal environment [21]. Due to the remoteness of its habitats, and the challenging environmental conditions and long periods of polar night throughout most of its range, detailed behavioural and matching environmental data covering several years with complete seasonal cycles were thus far lacking. In this study, we take advantage of a unique data set of multi-year movements of 19 female muskoxen tracked with GPS (Global Positioning System) collars in northeast Greenland (28 muskox-years, with 153–1062 observation days/animal) and apply HMMs to infer likely behavioural states (resting, foraging, relocating) from step lengths and turning angles between hourly positions. Our objectives were to (a) quantify diel, seasonal, and interannual variation in activity budgets and to (b) investigate how behavioural time allocation and state-switching are influenced by environmental conditions, using an extensive set of spatially explicit and/or temporally dynamic environmental variables. Finally, we also aimed to (c) evaluate whether the emerging behavioural patterns follow qualitative predictions derived from upscaled OFT. To do so, we formulated season-specific predictions for each of the foraging strategies proposed by OFT (see Table 1) against which we compared our results.

Table 1 Predictions for expected patterns in time allocation, state occupancy probabilities and activity scheduling if muskoxen were to follow either of the three proposed strategies according to optimal foraging theory, for the summer and winter season, respectively

Methods

Study area and study species

The study area in northeast Greenland (approx. 5000 km2; Fig. 1 a-b) is characterised by a high-arctic climate with pronounced variability in light and weather conditions throughout the year. The mean annual ambient temperature is − 9 °C (1997–2017), peaking in July (monthly mean of 6.6 °C) and dropping to lowest values in February/March (− 20 °C). Snow typically covers the ground from early September to early June, but interannual variation in snow accumulation and duration of snow cover is considerable [22]. Broad valleys separated by fjords and mountains up to 1600 m in elevation determine the landscape’s topography. The vegetation consists of different tundra habitat types of varying productivity [23]. Muskoxen are the only large herbivores in the study area. Considered sedentary [24], the resident population was estimated to range between 2900 and 4600 individuals in 1990 [25], with mean annual densities varying between 1 and 3 individuals km− 2 between 1996 and 2013 [26]. In summer, muskoxen mainly consume energy-rich graminoids [27, 28], whereas shrubs, but also graminoids, are important in winter [29, 30]. Winter diets may vary substantially between years depending on snow conditions [28], but also over the course of the winter, with lower diet quality in late as compared to early winter [30].

Fig. 1
figure 1

a Map indicating the study area (black rectangle) in northeast Greenland (white). b Map of the study area in detail (WGS 84, UTM zone 27), showing the distribution of landcover types (note that in the statistical models, lakes, non-vegetated and bare ground were pooled to ‘bare ground’). For the distribution of remaining static covariates, see Additional file 1: Fig. S1. c Muskox tracks during the snow-free summer and d snow-covered winter period across years, colour-coded by animal ID (within season). For an overview of muskox observations per season and year, see Additional file 1: Figs. S2-S3

Collection of muskox movement data

In October of 2013 and 2015, n = 14 and n = 5 adult female muskoxen were fitted with GPS collars (Tellus Large; Followit Lindesberg AB, Sweden), respectively (for a detailed description of collaring procedures, see [24, 31]). Note that a total of 15 females were collared in 2015, but 10 of the collars, fitted with a GPS unit from a different manufacturing batch, exhibited lower positional accuracy (location error of 19.6 m (SD = 25.4) as opposed to 8.04 m (SD = 7.23)) and were therefore excluded from analyses to minimise misclassification of behavioural states. Collars were programmed to record one position per hour.

Processing of movement data

Hourly location data were screened for impossible movements (detailed in [24]). Collar fix rate was generally high (98.5%), but in some instances, GPS fixes could not be obtained for several consecutive hours. If observation gaps exceeded 10 h, we split the movement tracks (n = 19) into bursts (n = 32) to keep time-series regularization (required for HMMs) and consequently needed interpolation of environmental covariate values for missing observations to a minimum. To account for potential snow-related differences in movement characteristics (e.g. shorter step length when moving in snow), bursts were subsequently divided into ‘seasonal’ bursts on the individual level: bursts were characterised as ‘snow-free’ (hereafter referred to as ‘summer’) for the period between the first and last 48 h where an individual did not encounter snow, and otherwise as ‘snow-covered’ (‘winter’) (see environmental data section on how snow conditions were acquired). Indeed, mean step lengths were over 80% longer in summer vs. winter (155.2 m v. 85.7 m, respectively). All seasonal bursts with less than 4 full weeks of consecutive observations were excluded from analyses (n = 7). Combined, 13.03% of observations were removed from the initial GPS dataset, and a total of 242,378 observations (nsummer = 28,165, nwinter = 214,213) divided into 70 seasonal bursts (Additional file 1: Fig. S2) were included in the final analyses, corresponding to 28 muskox-years with 153–1062 observation days/animal. For an overview of observations per season and animal ID, see Fig. 1 C-D and Additional file 1: Fig. S3/Table S1.

Environmental data

To assess how muskox time allocation and state-switching probabilities were influenced by environmental conditions, we considered a wide range of spatially explicit and/or temporally dynamic environmental covariates known to influence ungulate behaviour (e.g. [4, 32]), detailed in Table 2. The temporally dynamic and spatially explicit meteorological covariates (air temperature, snow depth, wind speed, wind direction, total precipitation, see Table 2) were modelled for the study area and period using SnowModel and MicroMet (see [34] for details). Values for all covariates were extracted based on muskox GPS positions, and associated timestamps for temporally dynamic covariates. While muskox locations were recorded hourly, temporally dynamic covariates were available at a 3-hourly or daily resolution, respectively. However, assuming that meteorological conditions do not change substantially within 3 h and given that muskoxen might change position in space and thereby encounter different covariate values (i.e. spatial variation), we decided not to resample the muskox location data. Snow depth was only included in the analyses of winter bursts, whereas NDVI was only available in the absence of snow and hence solely included as covariate in the analyses of summer bursts. As NDVI reflected temporal dynamics in NDVI over the course of the summer season, we included it in addition to the temporally static NDVI-based landcover classification (see Table 2) to investigate if/how behavioural variation in muskoxen was related to seasonal changes in vegetation greenness. Any missing covariate values were filled in by linear interpolation. Considered covariates did not exhibit collinearity issues (i.e. Pearson correlation coefficient < |0.6|).

Table 2 Overview of covariates considered in the HMMs for the snow-free summer and snow-covered winter bursts

Statistical analyses

To analyse muskox movement patterns over time, we fitted separate bivariate HMMs to the hourly-observed step lengths and turning angles for the summer and winter bursts, respectively. HMMs assume the observed movement patterns to be driven by an underlying latent state sequence (i.e. a finite-state Markov chain). These data-driven states can be interpreted as proxies for the animals’ unobserved behavioural modes [16]. Step lengths and turning angles were modelled using gamma and von Mises distributions, respectively, in each case conditional on the underlying state. As neither observed movement variables (Additional file 1: Fig. S4) nor initially explored modelled state-dependent distributions (Additional file 1: Fig. S5) revealed large differences between individuals’ movement patterns, we did not explicitly account for individual variation between animals in the HMMs. To assess potential spatio-temporal association between individuals (i.e. joint movements), we calculated the percentage of simultaneous positions where two animals were less than 100 m apart (group definition provided in [26]), using the global proximity analysis as implemented in the wildlifeDI R package. On average, 0.68% [range: 0–22%] of simultaneous summer and 0.67% [0–24%] of simultaneous winter GPS fixes were located within the defined distance threshold, and we therefore considered individuals to move independently of each other (see also Additional file 1: Fig. S2 [24];).

We used the HMMs to detect and classify the three most commonly observed major behavioural states within the daily activity patterns of muskoxen and other ungulate species (e.g. [32, 36]), namely resting (state 1), foraging (state 2) and relocating (state 3). We also explored HMMs with two, four and five states (Additional file 1: Figs. S6-S7), but although the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) favoured the 5-state model, we found that the HMMs with three states were the most reliable to interpret in a biologically meaningful way while still providing good model fit (Additional file 1: Fig. S9; for a more detailed discussion of state-selection in HMMs, see [37]). To investigate the influence of environmental conditions on muskox movement behaviour, the state transition probabilities were expressed as functions of the covariates using a multinomial logit link function with categories representing the different states the process might switch to [15]. Forward selection based on BIC was used to determine the influence of 14 covariates considered in each of the seasonal HMMs. To capture their periodic nature, we included sine and cosine terms for cyclic covariates (e.g. time of day). All HMMs were fitted in R (version 3.6.0 [38]) via numerical likelihood maximisation using the moveHMM package [39]. To avoid local maxima, we fitted each model with 30 sets of random starting values and chose the one with the highest log-likelihood value in each case.

Based on the final seasonal models, we decoded the latent states using the Viterbi algorithm [40], which provides the most likely state sequence given the model and thus the basis for calculating activity budgets and duration of behavioural bouts. Furthermore, for each of the covariates, we calculated the stationary probabilities of state occupancy as a function of the covariate values [41], with the other continuous covariates held fixed at their respective seasonal means, and the categorical covariates set to a reference category.

Approximation of foraging strategies

To evaluate whether the observed patterns in time allocation, state occupancy probabilities and activity scheduling followed either of the three foraging strategies (energy intake maximisation, time minimisation, net energy maximisation) proposed by OFT, we formulated qualitative season-specific predictions (Table 1) against which we compared our results (Table 3).

Table 3 Summary of how results support predictions (Table 1) for expected patterns in time allocation, state occupancy probabilities and activity scheduling if muskoxen were to follow either of the three proposed strategies according to optimal foraging theory, for the summer and winter season, respectively

Results

State-allocation and goodness-of-model-fit

The estimated state-dependent distribution for step lengths and turning angles differed between summer and winter (Fig. 2), but states generally followed the same pattern: State 1 was characterised by short step lengths and high turning angles (i.e. undirected movements), presumed to represent resting behaviour. State 2 included medium step lengths and turning angles centred around zero but with low concentration (i.e. low kappa) around the mean (indicating tortuous movements with a slight overall tendency for forwards movements). State 3 was associated with larger step lengths and turning angles highly concentrated around zero, representative of very directed movements. We thus assumed states 2 and 3 to reflect foraging and relocating behaviour, respectively. This interpretation is corroborated by Fig. 3 a-b, displaying example time series of step lengths and associated decoded states for both seasons. Mean step lengths were generally smaller during winter than summer (Fig. 2), supporting the separate modelling of the two seasons. The model-induced marginal distributions of the two movement variables corresponded well with the underlying empirical distributions (Fig. 2). For more details on model evaluation, see Additional file 1: Figs. S8-S9. The forward covariate selection procedure produced different results for summer and winter (Additional file 1: Fig. S10).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Histograms of step length and turning angle between hourly relocations, respectively, for the summer a, b and winter c, d season, overlaid with the state-dependent distributions as estimated by the HMMs selected by BIC. The state-dependent distributions were weighted according to the proportion of time spent in the different states, as inferred by the Viterbi sequence. Dashed black lines indicate the associated marginal observation distributions. Note that the x- and y-axes for step length were truncated at the upper range limit to facilitate visualisation (maximum observed step length was 3486 m for summer, and 3897 m for winter). Tables included in panels provide parameter estimates per state and model (mean step length with standard deviation; mean turning angle (phi) and angle concentration (kappa))

Fig. 3
figure 3

Example of state-decoded step lengths for the a summer and b winter season, showing a period of 18 days for one individual female, respectively. For all state-decoded muskox locations, see Additional file 1: Fig. S15. c Monthly boxplots for the individual-based mean duration of behavioural bouts

Behavioural time allocation during summer

For the summer season, the final model included light, landcover type, terrain ruggedness and Julian day as covariates (Additional file 1: Fig. S10), i.e. predominantly covariates reflecting variation in forage quality/quantity, supporting predictions S1INTAKE and S2INTAKE (Table 3). Muskox females spent an average of 69% [range: 63–74%] of time foraging, 19% resting [15–23%], and 12% relocating [7–19%] during summer (Additional file 1: Fig. S11 A), with the highest mean time spent foraging in August (66% [55–73%]), and only about 14% of time spent resting in June [10–21%] and July [10–17%] (Fig. 4 a). Time allocated to foraging was lowest on bare ground and highest in dense vegetation, and vice versa for relocating (Fig. 4 d). The probability of foraging increased with increasing terrain ruggedness (i.e. a proxy for vegetation heterogeneity, see Table 2), whereas probability of relocating decreased (Fig. 5 a). Neither landcover type nor terrain ruggedness strongly affected the probability of resting. Light seemed to have the strongest effect in summer, with muskoxen allocating more time to resting during darkness (32% [14–43%]) than daylight (18% [15–21%], Additional file 1: Fig. S11 B). Most observations included in the summer model were recorded during the midnight sun period, hence muskoxen were more likely to switch to the resting state during dark hours as soon as the sun began to set again (mid-August onwards). As evident from the covariate selection process and in line with predictions S3INTAKE and S4INTAKE (Table 3), neither time of day nor year explained much of the behavioural variation. Accordingly, no specific scheduling of daily activity could be detected during the midnight sun period (comprising most observations), and interannual differences in time allocation between summer seasons appeared minimal (Fig. 4 b-c).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Behavioural time allocation in female muskoxen in northeast Greenland depending on a day of the year, aggregated by month, b time of day during different light seasons (polar night = period of 24 h of darkness, midnight sun = period of 24 h daylight), c year and d landcover type (bare ground, sparse or dense vegetation). Note that in c year t denotes the winter season t-1 to t, i.e. for instance 2014 is the winter 2013–2014. For behavioural time allocation by Julian day, i.e. not aggregated by month, see Additional file 1: Fig. S11 D

Fig. 5
figure 5

Stationary probabilities (mean and 95% CI) of behavioural state occupancy as a function of the environmental covariates included in the final HMMs for the a summer and b-f winter season. According to BIC model selection, the final summer model included light, landcover type, terrain ruggedness and Julian day as covariates; the final winter model included Julian day, time of day, landcover type, terrain ruggedness, snow depth, light, ambient temperature, year, distance to coast and wind speed. Probabilities were calculated for each covariate and state by fixing the values of the remaining continuous environmental covariates at their respective seasonal mean. Continuous temporal covariates were set to Julian Day 213 (i.e. August 1st) and 91 (i.e. April 1st) for summer and winter, respectively, and to12 o’clock for time of day. Categorical covariates were set to their corresponding reference categories, i.e. to bare ground (landcover type), daylight, and, for the winter model, winter 2013–2014 (year). Monte Carlo simulation from the estimator’s approximate multivariate normal distribution was used to obtain pointwise 95% CIs. Coefficients of the multinomial logistic regression underlying this figure, as well as figures for probabilities of behavioural state occupancy for different categories (e.g. sparse/dense vegetation, darkness), are provided in the supplementary materials (Additional file 1: Tables S2-S3, Figs. S12-S14)

Behavioural time allocation during winter

The final winter model included Julian day, time of day, landcover type, terrain ruggedness, snow depth, light, ambient temperature, year, distance to coast and wind speed as covariates (Additional file 1: Fig. S10). In line with prediction W1NET, these covariates reflect variation in both forage as well as potentially constraining environmental conditions (Table 3). However, as time of day was selected as well, support for prediction W1TIME was likewise indicated. The average proportion of time spent in the foraging state during winter was lower (45% [34–50%]), and time spent resting about twice as high as during summer (43% [37–3%]) (Additional file 1: Fig. S11 A). Time spent foraging dropped to lowest mean value of 34% in both February [20–46%] and March [19–45%], and time spent resting was highest in January (54% [46–62%]) (Fig. 4 a).

Similar to the summer model, covariates representing forage quality/quantity/accessibility (landcover, ruggedness, distance to coast, see Table 2) were positively related to the probability of foraging, and probability of relocating increased with decreasing probability of foraging. Muskoxen spent most time foraging in dense vegetation, and more time relocating on bare ground (Fig. 4 D). Increasing terrain ruggedness – in winter a proxy for heterogeneity in both snow accumulation and vegetation (Table 2) – increased the probability of foraging but decreased the probability of relocating (Fig. 5 b). Responses to environmental conditions constituting potential energetic constraints (snow depth, ambient temperatures, wind speed) generally supported prediction W2NET (Table 3). When encountering deep snow, muskoxen were more likely to be resting, and less likely to be foraging or relocating (Fig. 5 c). Low ambient temperatures and high windspeeds likewise increased the probability of resting (Fig. 5 d, f). As opposed to the midnight sun period, distinct daily activity patterns were observed for the rest of the year (Fig. 4 b). During the polar night and the period of increasing light, pronounced unimodal activity peaks were apparent, whereas a bimodal pattern was exhibited during the period of decreasing light. These results are in accordance with prediction W3TIME (Table 3). Although year was selected as covariate, activity budgets did not show pronounced interannual variation between winter seasons (Fig. 4 c), lending some support for both W4INTAKE and W4NET (Table 3).

Duration of behavioural bouts

Throughout the year, duration of foraging and relocating bouts remained relatively constant (foraging (mean [range]) = 4.2 h [3.5–5 h]; relocating = 4 h [3.2–4.6 h]) (Fig. 3 c). Resting bout duration, however, varied markedly over the course of the year (mean = 3.6 h), with longest bouts in March (5.8 h [4.7–6.8 h]) and shortest in July (1.2 h [1.1–1.4 h]) (Fig. 3 c). This result is indicative of S1INTAKE and either W1INTAKE or W2NET (Table 3).

Discussion

Understanding how environmental conditions shape the foraging behaviour of free-ranging animals is a fundamental aspect of ecology, as foraging tactics influence individuals’ fitness and thus ultimately population dynamics [42, 43]. However, assessing animals’ foraging strategies and testing their optimality over time and space is challenging, especially for species where detailed year-round observations and direct assessments of foraging efforts and gains are not feasible. Muskoxen inhabit one of the most remote and seasonally extreme environments of this planet, and our study provides the first detailed account of behavioural strategies covering the full seasonal cycle over multiple years with pronounced variation in environmental conditions.

Optimising energy intake? Foraging strategies during summer

As capital breeders [44], muskoxen rely heavily on body reserves gained during the short forage-abundant summer to secure winter survival and calf production [45, 46] – even more so than reindeer/caribou (Rangifer tarandus), the only other arctic ungulate species [44]. In agreement, we found muskox time allocation during summers to be influenced primarily by forage and light conditions. Activity budgets indicated almost continuous foraging (Fig. 4 a), interrupted only by short resting and relocating periods (Fig. 3 c), with no specific daily scheduling of activities (Fig. 4 b) and little interannual variation (Fig. 4 c). These results clearly indicate that muskoxen in northeast Greenland follow an energy intake maximisation strategy during summer, as previously suggested by studies based on direct behavioural observations for other muskox populations [47,48,49].

Muskoxen allocated substantially more time to foraging during summer than winter (Fig. 4 a, Additional file 1: Fig. S11 A), mainly as a result of drastically shorter resting bouts (Fig. 3 c). This seems to be a widespread behavioural pattern observed in muskox populations elsewhere [3, 49], but also other ungulate species [2, 4, 50]. As high digestibility of plant material decreases time required for rumination [50], the short resting bouts likely reflect abundant high-quality forage. Indeed, digestibility and quality of summer forage is generally high in the Arctic [49, 51], and terrain heterogeneity and associated differences in plant phenology allow for selective foraging of vegetation in early growth stages (i.e. with highest digestibility) until late in the growing season [49]. The positive effect of terrain ruggedness on foraging activity (Fig. 5 a) was equally evident from our results.

Heat stress is a well-known constraint to animals’ activity [8, 32], especially in cold-adapted species [4]. Although muskoxen may be susceptible to high temperatures, physiological and behavioural limits to heat tolerance are unclear [52]. According to our results, muskox foraging behaviour appears unconstrained by heat stress given the experienced summer temperatures (up to 16.4 °C).

Digesting or conserving energy? Foraging strategies during winter

While arctic ungulates may heavily rely on body reserves gained during summer, foraging strategies during the long winters have to supplement summer reserves and may consequently be likewise important for survival [53], foetus development [54] and maintenance of the rumen microbiome [55]. We found partial support for all three tested OFT strategies (Table 3), hence our results are not easily reconcilable with just one dominant foraging strategy.

Evidently, muskoxen were less active (i.e. foraging or relocating) and rested more during winter (Fig. 4 a, Additional file 1: Fig. S11 A), with considerably longer resting bouts (Fig. 3 c). This increase in resting time is likely a functional response to the reduction in forage quality, requiring more time for digestion [50], providing some support for an energy intake maximisation strategy. As in summer, forage conditions (i.e. landcover type, ruggedness) strongly influenced behavioural time allocation in winter, with suboptimal foraging conditions apparently motivating relocation to different areas (Fig. 5 b).

However, behavioural time allocation and state-switching were also, albeit to a lesser degree, influenced by environmental conditions reflecting potential energetic constraints: Muskoxen reduced foraging activity and were more likely to rest when encountering conditions leading to heat loss (low ambient temperatures, high wind speeds, Fig. 5 d, f) or increased costs of foraging and movement (deep snow, Fig. 5 c). This finding, suggesting attempts to conserve energy, is indicative of a net energy maximisation strategy and rather contradicts a strategy of energy intake maximisation. Similar responses to adverse weather conditions have been found for muskoxen in West Greenland [47] as well as other ungulate species [2, 4], and inactivity and resting while lying down have been shown to reduce the metabolic costs of thermoregulation in cold weather [56]. However, given our movement data at an hourly resolution, we are currently not able to distinguish ‘true’ resting (i.e. time that would otherwise be available for foraging) from ruminating (dictated by digestive constraints). To determine to what degree ruminants living in highly seasonal environments allocate time to digestion versus energy conservation, future studies should attempt to more finely distinguish these two behaviours.

Diel activity patterns

Throughout the year, muskoxen were active during all hours of the day, but more likely to rest during darkness (Fig. 4 b, Additional file 1: Fig. S11 B). At the latitude of our study area (74 ° N), distinct cycles of daylight and darkness only occur for 12.5 weeks in spring and autumn, respectively. Nonetheless, we found distinct scheduling of daily activity for all seasons except for the period of continuous daylight (Fig. 4 b). However, predation risk and human disturbance are extremely low in our study area [26], and we therefore consider these factors unlikely determinants of activity scheduling. Hence, we argue against time minimisation as dominant foraging strategy in this population. Instead, we interpret these results as a clear expression of basic ultradian ruminant activity patterns, as also observed in predator-free Svalbard reindeer at similar latitudes [2, 57]. Moreover, time spent resting increased steadily from the end of summer towards highest levels in mid-winter (January to March), with a concomitant decrease in foraging time (Fig. 4 a). As the quality of muskox diets in the study area decreases significantly over winter [30], this result provides additional evidence against a time minimising strategy.

Limitations and future prospects

Previous studies assessing muskox activity patterns based on direct behavioural observations [3, 36, 47,48,49, 58] were of small geographic coverage and biased towards summers and daylight hours. Coupling telemetry data with hidden Markov modelling, we overcome these observational challenges, allowing us to gain detailed year-round insights into this key arctic herbivore’s behavioural variation. Our quantification of season-specific time allocation and identification of foraging constraints constitutes a critical step towards direct tests of optimal foraging strategies [32]. Clearly, combining location data with more precise estimates of availability, quality and type of consumed forage allows for a more direct assessment of obtained energetic benefits and thus foraging strategies [12], and hence constitutes a logical next step to complement our approach.

As we used an unsupervised HMM approach based on the distribution of step lengths and turning angles and not validated by direct observations of the animals, state classification should be interpreted with caution, i.e. only be considered as proxy for the underlying ‘true’ animal behaviour [59]. We limited our classification to the three most common, straightforward to interpret behavioural states, as recommended in an unsupervised HMM framework [37]. The state characterisations corresponded well to those described for other large herbivores [60, 61]. The resting state reflected minimal movements and uniformly distributed turning angles (with a somewhat higher probability of being directed towards 180° due to an artefact of GPS error). The foraging state was associated with slow, often tortuous movements resulting from alternate stationary foraging and intermittent small relocations when searching for suitable foraging spots. The relocation state was characterised by the longest steps and highest directional persistence. Muskoxen specifically have been found to move between 1.6 and 5.8 m/min while foraging, depending on season and vegetation type encountered [47]. Aggregated to an hourly scale, these values correspond well to the HMM-derived distribution of step lengths in state 2 (i.e. foraging) for summer and winter (Fig. 2). Obviously, muskoxen express a more nuanced range of behaviours than the three behavioural states considered here. However, due to the hourly resolution of our data, many behaviours can fall ‘under the radar’, i.e. the model-derived behavioural state classification can only provide a rough estimate of hourly behaviour and can generally not account for short-term or non-exclusive behaviours, such as social interactions or short foraging ‘pit-stops’ during larger-scale relocation bouts.

Nonetheless, even the interpretation of a 3-state model provides challenges. For example, muskoxen typically did not move extensively within an hour (see Fig. 2), making the distinction between resting and foraging behaviour difficult at times. Misclassifications may for instance happen during winter when the necessity to crater through snow to reach the underlying vegetation limits displacement during foraging, leading to an overestimation of resting time. This may have contributed to the fact that we were unable to determine a dominant fitting OFT strategy for winter. However, cratering behaviour appears confined to rather shallow snow depths [62], and muskoxen were found to rest longer as snow thickness at feeding craters increased [58]. These observations support the overall trend of decreasing foraging probability with increasing snow depth in our study. In summer, when switching between resting and foraging is highly volatile, short resting bouts (i.e. below 1 h) in particular may be masked, potentially leading to an overestimation of foraging time. Overall, however, we are confident that state classification in our HMMs is robust and captures most of the variation in muskox movement behaviour. A validated supervised classification approach would obviously help to quantify and/or rule out uncertainties related to misclassification, but was not feasible in the context of our study as direct observations were not possible in winter, i.e. for most of the year. Future studies could further refine state estimation by coupling GPS location data with more fine-scaled, continuous acceleration or other biologging data using hierarchical HMMs [63].

While our ability to collect fine-scale movement data is rapidly increasing, such data needs to be matched with environmental data at relevant temporal and spatial scales. Here, we tested a wide range of covariates, including rarely available temporally dynamic and spatially explicit covariates at the highest spatial resolution (300 m) and temporal frequency (3 h and daily) possible for this region. Although these covariates should in theory most directly represent the conditions influencing individuals and consequently explain much of the behavioural variation, this was only partially reflected in the results of the covariate selection process. This could signal a real biological effect, suggesting that muskoxen, well-adapted to extreme weather variability, are not particularly sensitive to dynamically changing environmental conditions. It may, however, also indicate that in highly heterogeneous environments, temporally static proxies for resource and climate gradients at high spatial resolution (e.g. landcover, ruggedness) may outperform temporally dynamic direct variables at coarser scales (e.g. NDVI). Given the importance of vegetation and snow conditions for alpine and arctic ungulates, further improvements in vegetation mapping as well as observations and modelling of wildlife-relevant snow variables [20, 35] are critical for detailed assessments of how animals respond to environmental variability.

Conclusions

By combining unique data sets of GPS-based movements and spatiotemporal environmental covariates within HMMs, we tested a series of qualitative predictions derived from upscaled OFT. We conclude that during the brief high-arctic summers, muskox females adopt an energy intake maximisation strategy, largely unconstrained by environmental conditions. For the long winter season, our results indicate partial support for all three tested foraging strategies. However, deep snow, low ambient temperatures and strong winds were clearly constraining foraging behaviour in winter (Fig. 5), with muskoxen instead allocating more time to resting, likely to conserve energy.

With climate change altering seasonal patterns and thus challenging species’ behavioural rhythms and adaptations [64, 65], assessing how environmental conditions shape the foraging behaviour of free-ranging animals is an increasingly urgent task. In environments as marginal as the high Arctic, where rapid warming is already impacting the entire biophysical system [66], even small deviations from optimal foraging behaviour may potentially have large consequences for reproductive success and survival, and thus ultimately population dynamics. Providing novel insights into the current relationship between environmental conditions and muskox behaviour, this study improves our understanding of how ungulate species are able to survive under highly variable conditions via adaptive fine-scale behaviour and lays the foundation for assessing behavioural plasticity for future adaptation.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets supporting the conclusions of this article are available in the Zenodo repository, https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.3768080.

Abbreviations

BIC:

Bayesian Information Criterion

GPS:

Global Positioning System

HMM:

hidden Markov model

NDVI:

Normalized Difference Vegetation Index

OFT:

Optimal foraging theory

References

  1. Bunnell FL, Harestadt AS. Activity budgets and body weight in mammals: how sloppy can mammals be? In: Genoways HH, editor. Curr Mammal. New York: Plenum Publishing Corp; 1989. p. 243–305.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Loe LE, Bonenfant C, Mysterud A, Severinsen T, Øritsland NA, Langvatn R, et al. Activity pattern of arctic reindeer in a predator-free environment: no need to keep a daily rhythm. Oecologia. 2007;152:617–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Côté SD, Schaefer JA, Messier F. Time budgets and synchrony of activities in muskoxen: the influence of sex, age, and season. Can J Zool. 1997;75:1628–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Brivio F, Bertolucci C, Tettamanti F, Filli F, Apollonio M, Grignolio S. The weather dictates the rhythms: Alpine chamois activity is well adapted to ecological conditions. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2016;70:1291–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Kie JG. The effects of cattle grazing on optimal foraging in mule deer (Odocoileus hemionus). For Ecol Manag. 1996;88:131–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Schoener TW. Theory of feeding strategies. Annu Rev Ecol Syst. 1971;2:369–404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Stephens DW, Krebs JR. Foraging theory. Princeton University Press; 1986.

  8. Owen-Smith N, Goodall V. Coping with savanna seasonality: comparative daily activity patterns of African ungulates as revealed by GPS telemetry. J Zool. 2014;293:181–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Kohli M, Sankaran M, Suryawanshi KR, Mishra C. A penny saved is a penny earned: lean season foraging strategy of an alpine ungulate. Anim Behav. 2014;92:93–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Johnson DH. The comparison of usage and availability measurements for evaluating resource preference. Ecology. 1980;61:65–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Johnson CJ, Parker KL, Heard DC. Foraging across a variable landscape: behavioral decisions made by woodland caribou at multiple spatial scales. Oecologia. 2001;127:590–602.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Owen-Smith N, Fryxell JMM, Merrill EHH. Foraging theory upscaled: the behavioural ecology of herbivore movement. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2010;365:2267–78.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Polansky L, Douglas-Hamilton I, Wittemyer G. Using diel movement behavior to infer foraging strategies related to ecological and social factors in elephants. Mov Ecol. 2013;1:1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Hebblewhite M, Haydon DT. Distinguishing technology from biology: a critical review of the use of GPS telemetry data in ecology. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2010;365:2303–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Patterson TA, Parton A, Langrock R, Blackwell PG, Thomas L, King R. Statistical modelling of individual animal movement: an overview of key methods and a discussion of practical challenges. AStA Adv Stat Anal. 2017;101:399–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Langrock R, King R, Matthiopoulos J, Thomas L, Fortin D, Morales JM. Flexible and practical modeling of animal telemetry data: hidden Markov models and extensions. Ecology. 2012;93:2336–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Liston GE, Elder K. A distributed snow-evolution modeling system (SnowModel). J Hydrometeorol. 2006;7:1259–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Liston GE, Elder K. A meteorological distribution system for high-resolution terrestrial modeling (MicroMet). J Hydrometeorol. 2006;7:217–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Mahoney PJ, Liston GE, LaPoint S, Gurarie E, Mangipane B, Wells AG, et al. Navigating snowscapes: scale-dependent responses of mountain sheep to snowpack properties. Ecol Appl 2018;0:1–15.

  20. Boelman NT, Liston GE, Gurarie E, Meddens AJH, Mahoney PJ, Kirchner PB, et al. Integrating snow science and wildlife ecology in Arctic-boreal North America. Environ Res Lett. 2019;14:010401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Berger J, Hartway C, Gruzdev A, Johnson M. Climate degradation and extreme icing events constrain life in cold-adapted mammals. Sci Rep. 2018;8:1–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Pedersen SH, Tamstorf MP, Abermann J, Westergaard-Nielsen A, Lund M, Skov K, et al. Spatiotemporal characteristics of seasonal snow cover in Northeast Greenland from in situ observations. Arctic, Antarct Alp Res. 2016;48:653–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Arndal MF, Illeris L, Michelsen A, Albert K, Tamstorf M, Hansen BU. Seasonal variation in gross ecosystem production, plant biomass, and carbon and nitrogen pools in five high Arctic vegetation types. Arctic, Antarct Alp Res. 2009;41:164–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Schmidt NM, van Beest FM, Mosbacher JB, Stelvig M, Hansen LH, Nabe-Nielsen J, et al. Ungulate movement in an extreme seasonal environment: year-round movement patterns of high-arctic muskoxen. Wildlife Biol. 2016;22:253–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Boertmann D, Forchhammer M, Olesen CR, Aastrup P, Thing H. The Greenland muskox population status 1990. Rangifer. 1992;12:5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Schmidt NM, Pedersen SH, Mosbacher JB, Hansen LH. Long-term patterns of muskox (Ovibos moschatus) demographics in high arctic Greenland. Polar Biol. 2015;38:1667–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Kristensen DK, Kristensen E, Forchhammer MC, Michelsen A, Schmidt NM. Arctic herbivore diet can be inferred from stable carbon and nitrogen isotopes in C3 plants, faeces, and wool. Can J Zool. 2011;89:892–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. Mosbacher JB, Michelsen A, Stelvig M, Hendrichsen DK, Schmidt NM. Show me your rump hair and I will tell you what you ate - the dietary history of muskoxen (Ovibos moschatus) revealed by sequential stable isotope analysis of guard hairs. PLoS One. 2016;11:1–13.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Klein DR, Bay C. Diet selection by vertebrate herbivores in the high Arctic of Greenland. Holarct Ecol. 1991;14:152–5.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Schmidt NM, Mosbacher JB, Vesterinen EJ, Roslin T, Michelsen A. Limited dietary overlap amongst resident Arctic herbivores in winter: complementary insights from complementary methods. Oecologia. 2018;187:689–99.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Grøndahl C, Andersen-Ranberg EU, Mosbacher JB, Stelvig M, Hansen LH, Schmidt NM. Immobilizing muskox (Ovibos Moschatus) under high Arctic conditions. J Zoo Wildl Med. 2018;49:856–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Belovsky GE, Slade JB. Time budgets of grassland herbivores: body size similarities. Oecologia. 1986;70:53–62.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Tamstorf MP, Illeris L, Hansen BU, Wisz M. Spectral measures and mixed models as valuable tools for investigating controls on land surface phenology in high arctic Greenland. BMC Ecol. 2007;7:9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Pedersen SH, Liston GE, Tamstorf MP, Abermann J, Lund M, Schmidt NM. Quantifying snow controls on vegetation greenness. Ecosphere. 2018;9:e02309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Pettorelli N, Vik JO, Mysterud A, Gaillard J-M, Tucker CJ, Stenseth NC. Using the satellite-derived NDVI to assess ecological responses to environmental change. Trends Ecol Evol. 2005;20:503–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Jingfors KT. Seasonal activity budgets and movements of a reintroduced Alaskan muskox herd. J Wildl Manag. 1982;46:344–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Pohle J, Langrock R, van Beest FM, Schmidt NM. Selecting the number of states in hidden Markov models: pragmatic solutions illustrated using animal movement. J Agric Biol Environ Stat. 2017;22:270–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Core R. Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical. Computing. 2019.

  39. Michelot T, Langrock R, Patterson TA. moveHMM : an R package for the statistical modelling of animal movement data using hidden Markov models. Methods Ecol Evol. 2016;7:1308–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Zucchini W, MacDonald IL, Langrock R. Hidden Markov models for time series: an introduction using R. second. Boca Raton: CRC Press; 2016.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Patterson TA, Basson M, Bravington MV, Gunn JS. Classifying movement behaviour in relation to environmental conditions using hidden Markov models. J Anim Ecol. 2009;78:1113–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. White RG. Foraging patterns and their multiplier effects on productivity of northern ungulates. Oikos. 1983;40:377–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. McLoughlin PD, Boyce MS, Coulson T, Clutton-Brock T. Lifetime reproductive success and density-dependent, multi-variable resource selection. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2006;273:1449–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Kerby J, Post E. Capital and income breeding traits differentiate trophic match-mismatch dynamics in large herbivores. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2013;368:20120484.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Adamczewski JZ, Fargey PJ, Laarveld B, Gunn A, Flood PF. The influence of fatness on the likelihood of early-winter pregnancy in muskoxen (Ovibos moschatus). Theriogenology. 1998;50:605–14.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. White R, Rowell J, Hauer W. The role of nutrition, body condition and lactation on calving success in muskoxen. J Zool. 1997;243:13–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Forchhammer MC. Sex, age, and seasonal variation in the foraging dynamics of muskoxen, Ovibos moschatus, in Greenland. Can J Zool. 1995;73:1344–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Forchhammer MC, Boomsma JJ. Foraging strategies and seasonal diet optimization of muskoxen in West Greenland. Oecologia. 1995;104:169–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Klein DR, Bay C. Foraging dynamics of muskoxen in Peary Land, northern Greenland. Ecography (Cop). 1990;13:269–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Cederlund G. Activity patterns in moose and roe deer in a north boreal forest. Ecography (Cop). 1989;12:39–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Larter NC, Nagy JA. Seasonal and annual variability in the quality of important forage plants on Banks Island, Canadian. High Arctic Appl Veg Sci. 2001;4:115–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Kutz S, Rowell J, Adamczewski J, Gunn A, Cuyler C, Aleuy OA, et al. Muskox health ecology symposium 2016: gathering to share knowledge on Umingmak in a time of rapid change. Arctic. 2017;70:225–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Tyler NJC. The relationship between the fat content of Svalbard reindeer in autumn and their death from starvation in winter. Rangifer. 1986;6:311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Desforges J-P, Marques GM, Beumer LT, Chimienti M, Blake J, Rowell JE, et al. Quantification of the full lifecycle bioenergetics of a large mammal in the high Arctic. Ecol Model. 2019;401:27–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Aagnes TH, Sørmo W, Mathiesen SD. Ruminal microbial digestion in free-living, in captive lichen-fed, and in starved reindeer (Rangifer tarandus tarandus) in winter. Appl Environ Microbiol. 1995;61:583–91.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Moen AN. Energy conservation by White-tailed deer in the winter. Ecology. 1976;57:192–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. van Oort BEH, Tyler NJC, Gerkema MP, Folkow L, Stokkan K-A. Where clocks are redundant: weak circadian mechanisms in reindeer living under polar photic conditions. Naturwissenschaften. 2007;94:183–94.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Schaefer JA, Messier F. Winter activity of muskoxen in relation to foraging conditions. Ecoscience. 1996;3:147–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Leos-Barajas V, Gangloff EJ, Adam T, Langrock R, van Beest FM, Nabe-Nielsen J, et al. Multi-scale modeling of animal movement and general behavior data using hidden Markov models with hierarchical structures. J Agric Biol Environ Stat. 2017;22:232–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Ditmer MA, Moen RA, Windels SK, Forester JD, Ness TE, Harris TR. Moose at their bioclimatic edge alter their behavior based on weather, landscape, and predators. Curr Zool. 2018;64:419–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Owen-Smith N, Traill LW. Space use patterns of a large mammalian herbivore distinguished by activity state: fear versus food? J Zool. 2017;303:281–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Kazmin VD, Abaturov BD. Mechanical properties of the snow cover and availability of forage for reindeer (Rangifer tarandus) and musk oxen (Ovibos moschatus) on pastures of Wrangel Island. Zool Zhurnal. 2009;88:990–1000.

    Google Scholar 

  63. Adam T, Griffiths CA, Leos-Barajas V, Meese EN, Lowe CG, Blackwell PG, et al. Joint modelling of multi-scale animal movement data using hierarchical hidden Markov models. Methods Ecol Evol. 2019;10:1536–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Pettorelli N, Pelletier F, von Hardenberg A, Festa-Bianchet M, Côté SD. Early onset of vegetation growth vs. rapid green-up: impacts on juvenile mountain ungulates. Ecology. 2007;88:381–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Plard F, Gaillard JM, Coulson T, Hewison AJM, Delorme D, Warnant C, et al. Mismatch between birth date and vegetation phenology slows the demography of roe deer. PLoS Biol. 2014;12:1–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  66. Box JE, Colgan WT, Christensen TR, Schmidt NM, Lund M, Parmentier FJW, et al. Key indicators of Arctic climate change: 1971-2017. Environ Res Lett. 2019;14:045010.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank the Greenland Ecosystem Monitoring Programme for access to ecosystem data, and Aarhus University, Denmark, for providing access to and logistics at Zackenberg. We are also grateful to Eliezer Gurarie and two anonymous reviewers for their very constructive feedback that helped to improve this article.

Funding

This study was financially supported by the Danish charity foundation 15. Juni Fonden, the Danish Environmental Protection Agency and the AUFF Starting Grant (AUFF-F-2o16-FLS-8-16, awarded to FMvB).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

LTB, NMS and FMvB conceived the idea and designed the study with input from JP and RL. The telemetry data was collected by NMS, LHH and MS. SHP provided modelled environmental covariate output. The data were analysed by LTB and JP, with suggestions from RL and FMvB. LTB wrote the manuscript and created the figures and tables, with significant contributions by JP and FMvB. All co-authors contributed to the manuscript, revised and commented subsequent drafts and gave final approval for publication.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Larissa T. Beumer.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Approval of ethical aspects was granted under the research permits issued by the Greenland Government, Ministry of Domestic Affairs, Nature and Environment (j.nos. G13–029 and G15–019).

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Additional file 1.

An application of upscaled optimal foraging theory using hidden Markov modelling: year-round behavioural variation in a large arctic herbivore.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Beumer, L.T., Pohle, J., Schmidt, N.M. et al. An application of upscaled optimal foraging theory using hidden Markov modelling: year-round behavioural variation in a large arctic herbivore. Mov Ecol 8, 25 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40462-020-00213-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40462-020-00213-x

Keywords