Skip to main content

Crabs ride the tide: incoming tides promote foraging of Giant Mud Crab (Scylla serrata)

Abstract

Background

Effective fisheries management of mobile species relies on robust knowledge of animal behaviour and habitat-use. Indices of behaviour can be useful for interpreting catch-per-unit-effort data which acts as a proxy for relative abundance. Information about habitat-use can inform stocking release strategies or the design of marine protected areas. The Giant Mud Crab (Scylla serrata; Family: Portunidae) is a swimming estuarine crab that supports significant fisheries harvest throughout the Indo-West Pacific, but little is known about the fine-scale movement and behaviour of this species.

Methods

We tagged 18 adult Giant Mud Crab with accelerometer-equipped acoustic tags to track their fine-scale movement using a hyperbolic positioning system, alongside high temporal resolution environmental data (e.g., water temperature), in a temperate south-east Australian estuary. A hidden Markov model was used to classify movement (i.e., step length, turning angle) and acceleration data into discrete behaviours, while also considering the possibility of individual variation in behavioural dynamics. We then investigated the influence of environmental covariates on these behaviours based on previously published observations.

Results

We fitted a model with two well-distinguished behavioural states describing periods of inactivity and foraging, and found no evidence of individual variation in behavioural dynamics. Inactive periods were most common (79% of time), and foraging was most likely during low, incoming tides; while inactivity was more likely as the high tide receded. Model selection removed time (hour) of day and water temperature (°C) as covariates, suggesting that they do not influence Giant Mud Crab behavioural dynamics at the temporal scale investigated.

Conclusions

Our study is the first to quantitatively link fine-scale movement and behaviour of Giant Mud Crab to environmental variation. Our results suggest Giant Mud Crab are a predominantly sessile species, and support their status as an opportunistic scavenger. We demonstrate a relationship between the tidal cycle and foraging that is likely to minimize predation risk while maximizing energetic efficiency. These results may explain why tidal covariates influence catch rates in swimming crabs, and provide a foundation for standardisation and interpretation of catch-per-unit-effort data—a commonly used metric in fisheries science.

Background

Animal movement is a fundamental ecological process whereby individual-level behaviours (e.g., foraging, migration) give rise to emergent properties of populations (e.g., growth rates, abundance, distribution; [1]). For mobile species, quantifying individual movement underpins effective fisheries management (reviewed in [2, 3]). For example, catch-per-unit-effort (or catch rate) data is commonly used as an index of abundance in stock assessment, assuming the probability of capture (i.e., catchability) is constant for all individuals [4, 5]. However, this is rarely the case and catchability is closely linked to patterns in foraging, especially in fisheries that employ baited gear [6]. For endothermic species, foraging is typically promoted by warmer temperatures due to increased metabolism [6]. To account for this and ensure catch rate data is a reliable index of abundance, so-called ‘catch rate standardization’ (i.e., some form of regression between catch rate and environmental covariates) must be undertaken to remove the effect of environmental variation [4, 5]. As such, identifying specific behaviours (e.g., foraging; [7, 8]) and quantifying the influence of environmental variation on them can be useful in standardization and interpretation of catch-per-unit-effort data [9]. Furthermore, assessing how these behaviours are allocated among different habitats can be used to inform the design of marine protected areas [10, 11] and abundance surveys, prioritize restoration of degraded habitats [12, 13] and target release locations for stocking of hatchery reared individuals.

Acoustic telemetry is a powerful method for quantifying the movement of marine organisms and has been broadly applied in fisheries research [2, 3, 14]. Recently, hyperbolic positioning systems have allowed researchers to track animals at increasingly fine spatiotemporal scales (e.g., metres and minutes; see Hyperbolic positioning for technical details; [15,16,17]). In addition, accelerometry has enabled detailed insight into the behavioural structure of animal movement [18, 19]. These technological advances have been accompanied by a proliferation of novel statistical approaches to analyze such data [20,21,22]. Hidden Markov models are stochastic time-series models that classify observed animal movement or accelerometry data into unobserved (or ‘hidden’) states, which can be interpreted as proxies for behavioural states of the tagged animal [23,24,25]. These models are a natural choice for analyzing animal movement [23, 25] and accelerometry [24], since they explicitly model the serial dependence structure that is typical of such data. Furthermore, hidden Markov models are highly flexible, and can be extended to model the influence of environmental covariates on animal behaviour (e.g., water temperature; [26]) while accounting for individual-level variation in behaviour (e.g., [27,28,29]).

The Giant Mud Crab (Scylla serrata) is a large portunid crab (Family: Portunidae) that is widely distributed throughout the Indo-West Pacific [30]. Crabs within this family are commonly referred to as the ‘swimming crabs’, due to their efficient swimming ability afforded by flattened paddles on their fifth legs (i.e., pleopods or swimmerets; [31]). In Australia, the species supports seasonal commercial and recreational fisheries [32, 33], with the main method of harvest being the deployment of baited traps (or pots; [34, 35]) during the austral spring–summer (Hewitt et al., unpublished data). Adult crabs inhabit sub- and intertidal habitats in estuaries, such as mudflats and mangroves [36, 37]. As a predominantly sessile species, daily movements occur at fine spatial scales (e.g., 219–910 m d−1; [38]) with high site fidelity [36], however adults are capable of long distance migrations (e.g., ~ 30–200 km; [39,40,41]). Adult crabs are carnivorous, opportunistic scavengers [42,43,44] and are generally thought to forage nocturnally [38, 43] using a combination of olfaction and contact chemoreception [44, 45]. Foraging is promoted by warmer temperatures (i.e., 25–30 °C; [46]), and stable isotope analysis suggests they derive their nutrition from a combination of seagrass [47, 48], mangrove [49] and saltmarsh habitats [48, 50]. Finally, for species that inhabit shallow waters, the tidal cycle imposes frequent changes in local conditions (e.g., the availability of intertidal foraging habitat). Tidal currents may also promote movement via selective tidal-stream transport [51]; a behaviour whereby individuals use tidal currents/flow to minimize the energetic costs associated with movement [52] that has been exhibited by Giant Mud Crab [39, 41, 53].

In this study, we sought to investigate the environmental drivers of fine-scale movement and behaviour of free-ranging adult Giant Mud Crab in a creek adjoining a temperate southeast Australian estuary. Specifically, this was achieved by: (1) tracking the movement of adult crabs with accelerometer-equipped acoustic tags using a hyperbolic positioning system; and (2) using a hidden Markov model to classify movement and acceleration data into discrete behaviours, and model transitions between these behaviours as a function of environmental covariates [24, 25].

Methods

Study site and array design

This study was conducted in Fenninghams Island Creek (32.75° S, 152.05° E), a small tributary to Port Stephens, a mature wave-dominated barrier estuary [54] situated on the temperate mid-north coast of New South Wales (NSW, Australia; Fig. 1). Fenninghams Island Creek is a narrow, relatively shallow creek (0.2–2 m depth) that encompasses typical estuarine habitats including unvegetated soft sediments (sub- and intertidal), seagrass (Zostera sp.), mangrove (Avicennia marina) and saltmarsh (Sporobolus virginicus, Sarcocornia quinqueflora and Suaeda australis; Fig. 1). It has a maximum tidal range of approximately 2 m, and mangrove and saltmarsh habitats are inundated twice daily (especially during spring tides). The study area is a ‘Sanctuary Zone’ within the Port Stephens Great Lakes Marine Park, which prohibits fishing or crab-trapping, allowing this study to proceed without the risk of fishing mortality or any effect of baited traps on movement. However, it is possible that some crabs may migrate in and out of the study area and be exposed to fishing in adjacent areas [39, 40]. Oyster farming is permitted, and tray cultivation is practiced along both shorelines of the creek (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Map of a Fenninghams Island Creek showing the locations of receivers and fixed-position reference tags within the array, as well as the distribution of seagrass, mangrove and saltmarsh. Oyster farming infrastructure is indicated by the grey outline. The location of Fenninghams Island Creek and within Port Stephens (inset; Sanctuary Zones in red), and b on the east Australian coast is indicated

A hyperbolic positioning system (hereafter referred to as the ‘array’), employing 10 Innovasea VR2W receivers and co-located synchronisation (or ‘sync’) tags (Innovasea, Nova Scotia, Canada) was established along approximately 500 m of Fenninghams Island Creek (Fig. 1). Range testing of similar arrays in comparable systems (e.g., [55, 56]) informed a receiver spacing of 100–200 m. Each receiver and sync tag were chained to existing infrastructure (e.g., oyster trays, jetty), or independent moorings consisting of a float and anchor attached by chain, approximately 0.3–0.4 m from the bottom. To monitor positional error throughout the study, two fixed-position reference tags (V9-2x-BLU-3), with identical programming as tags deployed on crabs (see Tag programming below), except with a longer random transmission interval (240–360 s), were deployed within and immediately adjacent to the array (Fig. 1). Since the position of these tags were known, the distance between the estimated position (see Hyperbolic positioning) and their actual position provides an indication of the performance of the array throughout the study [17]. The reference tag adjacent to the array stopped transmitting after one day, and was missing at the conclusion of the study, so only detections from the reference tag within the array were used in subsequent analysis. Water temperature (°C) and conductivity (mS cm−1) was monitored throughout the duration of the study using a HOBO U24-002-C conductivity/salinity logger (Onset Computer Corporation, Massachusetts, USA).

Receivers were retrieved and downloaded using VUE software (v. 2.6.2; Innovasea, Amirix, Nova Scotia, Canada) after the estimated battery life of the last tag deployed had passed (~ 7 months). Detection data was subsequently uploaded to the Integrated Marine Observing System Animal Tracking Facility (IMOS ATF; https://animaltracking.aodn.org.au; [3]) which was also interrogated for any additional tag detections outside of our array.

Crab capture and tagging

Giant Mud Crab were captured during the late austral summer (February 2020) using round collapsible mesh traps (i.e., pots; 0.9 m diameter × 0.27 m high), with 55 mm mesh and two semi-closed funnel entrances (0.25 × 0.05 m). Traps were deployed for approximately 24 h periods along 750 m of Fenninghams Island Creek (within and just upstream of the array). Captured crabs were cooled for 10–20 s in an ice/sea-water slurry (to decrease aggressive behaviour; following 39) and subsequently measured (to the nearest mm) for carapace length (CL; distance between the frontal notch and posterior carapace margin), sexed and moult-staged (following [57]). Only adult crabs (> 100 mm CL) that were likely to have recently moulted (i.e., post- or inter-moult; [57]) were tagged, to limit the probability of tag loss during ecdysis [58]. Innovasea V9A-2H accelerometer tags (hereafter ‘tags’; length: 43 mm, wet weight: 3.3 g; Innovasea, Nova Scotia, Canada) were affixed to the posterior carapace using instant adhesive (Loctite 406, Henkel Adhesives, Australia) which has shown tag retention of at least 3 months [39]. After tagging, crabs were gently submerged alongside the research vessel and once normal activity (e.g., attempted swimming) had resumed crabs were released within the bounds of the array. In general, negative impacts (e.g., stress, limb loss) are low for crabs handled and released in this manner [34]. During tagging, two previously tagged crabs were recaptured that had lost their tags, evidenced by adhesive present on the carapace. These crabs were re-tagged and data from the initial tags was excluded from our analysis (identified as continuous transmission from a single point), resulting in movement data from 18 crabs.

Tag programming

Tags were programmed to emit a unique signal (69 kHz) with high power output (151 dB re 1 µPa at 1 m) at random intervals between 150 and 210 s (180 s nominal). High power output was chosen in an attempt to overcome potential signal attenuation owing to burial of crabs [38, 46, 59] and the presence of seagrass within the study site [60]. Random signal transmission times were employed to minimize potential signal overlap (i.e., code-collision) which can block detection. Tags were equipped with an accelerometer programmed to record tri-axial acceleration data, which represents a general index of activity, analogous to overall dynamic body acceleration (ODBA; [61, 62]). Measurements are transmitted as a root mean square (RMS) acceleration vector with a range 0–3.4 m s−2 [63]. Since Giant Mud Crab are expected to be predominantly slow-moving [38, 43, 46], acceleration data was recorded at 5 Hz (i.e., 5 samples s−1) over a 20 s window to capture ‘bursts’ of acceleration (H. Pedersen, pers. comm.). See Taylor, McPhan [63] for a discussion of accelerometer programming. Battery life was estimated 196 d at these settings.

Hyperbolic positioning

Positions of tagged crabs were estimated by Innovasea using proprietary hyperbolic positioning algorithms. This approach estimates the position of tagged animals based on the time-difference-of-arrival (TDOA or multilateration) of detections at 3 or more receivers within an array. Assuming no measurement error, detection on a pair of receivers defines a hyperbola on which a tag may have been during signal transmission. Detection on a third receiver defines a second hyperbola, and the intersection of the two is the position of the tag [17]. Using this approach, the time of a detection is converted to distance based on signal propagation speed. Signal propagation speed was estimated via the Coppens equation [64] using measured water temperature (°C) and salinity and an assumed depth of 1.8 m, assuming ideal signal propagation (i.e., spreads spherically at a constant speed; [17]). Innovasea receiver clocks can drift by up to 4 s d−1 (dependent on water temperature) leading to differences in time among receiver clocks (i.e., clock skew). To account for this, detections from sync tags were used to calculate the skew between receiver clocks and synchronize detection times (see Smith [17] for technical details).

Overall, detections from our fixed-position reference tag indicated that location error within the array was low and positively-skewed for the duration of the study (Additional file 1: Fig. S1), with a mean error of 2.77 ± 9.24 m (SD) and median of 1.26 m (interquartile range = 0.78 m), and 93% of positions were within 5 m of their actual (known) location. In general, these results indicate that measurement error was low for the duration of the study.

Data processing

All subsequent analysis was undertaken using R (v. 4.0.2; [65]). Visual inspection suggested no ‘tagging effects’ (e.g., elevated/decreased activity) were apparent in the tagged population (Additional file 1: Fig. S2), and we included detections from the first day of tracking in our analysis to preserve our sample size. In general, hidden Markov models are formulated in discrete-time, meaning they require temporally regular observations [66], but see Glennie et al. [67]. Many factors can contribute to temporally irregular observations in our study, including temporary emigration from the array, burial [59] and random transmission intervals in our tags. To accommodate this, we predicted temporally regular locations at 5, 10 and 15-min intervals by modelling crab movement as a continuous-time correlated random walk using the R package ‘crawl’ [68]. We refer readers to Johnson, London [69] for a full mathematical description of this model. Before predicting temporally regular locations, detections were split into ‘tracks’ where the interval between detections were greater than 4 times the interpolation interval (i.e., 20, 40 and 60-min). This was to ensure we did not introduce unreasonable uncertainty or bias our data by consecutively predicting locations within these longer temporal gaps, which would result in straight and constant movement [70]. Furthermore, tracks with less than 100 detections were excluded, as those with few observations can give rise to issues with numerical stability (i.e., non-convergence), and typically reveal less about behavioural state dynamics [71]. Locations were estimated using a state-space framework, allowing incorporation of measurement error in location estimates [69]. This was achieved by transforming the error (in metres) along the longitudinal and latitudinal axes, derived from the fixed-position reference tag, into a covariance matrix and approximating it with a bivariate Gaussian distribution during model fitting [69]. Locations were estimated via maximum likelihood, and thus require initial estimates of parameter values. To ensure adequate exploration of the likelihood surface and convergence (to a global maxima) we used 50 random perturbations of the initial parameter values and retained output from the model with the highest log-likelihood [72].

Since we predicted locations less frequently (i.e., every 5-, 10-, 15-min) than the random transmission interval of our tags (i.e., every 4–6 min) it was possible that there were some intervals where no acceleration data was recorded as no detection was recorded but a location was predicted. Relatively few missing values is typically not an issue when fitting hidden Markov models [23], and the missing observations did not contribute to the likelihood during model fitting.

Behavioural state classification

Behaviour of tagged crabs was modelled using a hidden Markov model [73] via maximum likelihood using the R package ‘momentuHMM’ [72]. Hidden Markov models are stochastic time-series models with two components: an observable (possibly multivariate) state-dependent process, and an unobservable (‘hidden’) state-process [24, 25]. The state-dependent process consists of observed animal detections or metrics derived from them (e.g., step length, turning angle), while the state-process is a series of N-states, that are taken to represent the underlying behavioural modes of the animal [24, 25]. Two assumptions govern this model structure: (1) observations that comprise the state-dependent process are assumed to be conditionally independent, with the observation at time t conditional on the state at time t, and independent of all other states and observations; and (2) the state-process is a Markov chain, which means the probability of being in a given state at time t is completely determined by the state active at time t − 1. Transitions between states are governed by an N × N transition probability matrix, the entries in which denote the probability of switching states between time t and t + 1 [72], with entries on the main diagonal representing the probability of remaining in the same state (i.e., state-dwell probabilities; [24]). Hidden Markov models thereby link observed animal movement to unobserved (or ‘hidden’) underlying behavioural modes and provide a description of how they change through time [23,24,25].

In our case, observations that comprised the state-dependent processes included step length (m; i.e., distance moved) and mean acceleration (m s−2) between time t and t + 1 and turning angle (radians) between detections at t − 1, t and t + 1, where 0 radians corresponds to straight-line movement and ± π radians indicates course reversal. Step lengths and mean acceleration values were modelled using a zero-inflated gamma distribution, to account for instances where no movement occurred (i.e., step length = 0 m or acceleration = 0 m s−2; [72]). Note, the zero-inflated gamma distribution is defined only for non-negative real numbers (i.e., ≥ 0), and standard deviations greater than the mean reflect highly positively-skewed distributions and do not imply negative values. Turning angles were modelled using a wrapped Cauchy distribution, which is a probability distribution that results from ‘wrapping’ the Cauchy distribution around the unit circle, with a concentration parameter ranging between 0 and 1 that measures how concentrated turning angles are around the mean [72]. For each state, the mean (± standard deviation, SD) step length and acceleration was estimated using a log-link function, while turning angle mean was fixed at 0 radians (i.e., straight-line movement) and concentration was estimated using the logit-link function [72]. Initial parameter estimates were obtained using the same approach as with predicting temporally regular locations (see Data processing), whereby the fitting procedure was run 50 times with randomly selected initial values and output from the model with the highest log-likelihood was retained [72, 74]. A prior for the log-density of the working scale parameter distributions (N[0, 100]) was specified to avoid estimates near the boundary.

A central challenge when fitting hidden Markov models is deciding on the number of states (N) to estimate, which must be specified a priori, since traditional model selection techniques (e.g., minimizing information criteria) tend to select models that include more states than are biologically meaningful/interpretable [75, 76]. This is because state estimation is data-driven, meaning the estimated states may not correspond to a biologically meaningful behaviour, rather they provide proxies for them and require post-hoc interpretation [24, 25]. In this context, adding more states may simply be capturing random noise in the data rather than uncovering additional behavioural states. Pohle, Langrock [76] argue that the number of states should be chosen pragmatically, based on statistical and biological intuition. We expected Giant Mud Crab to exhibit 2–3 discrete behaviours, namely: inactivity/resting, foraging and possibly some inter-habitat migration [36, 38, 46, 77, 78], therefore we limited our analysis to two- and three-state models (i.e., N ϵ {2, 3}).

Behavioural state dynamics

Individual-level variation in behaviour is common among free-ranging animals, due to true differences (e.g., animal ‘personality’; [79]), different environmental contexts [80] or as an artefact of variable deployment lengths between individuals [28, 81]. This can be accommodated by including discrete-valued random effects (e.g., sex, individual) in a mixed hidden Markov model [27,28,29, 80]. To do so, K mixtures (K ϵ {1, …, 4}) were included in a ‘null’ model (i.e., without any environmental covariates), with crab ID as a discrete-valued random effect. Under this formulation, each K represents a distinct transition probability matrix allowing for up to 4 behavioural ‘types’ among individuals [28, 82]. For K = 1, behavioural dynamics are assumed to be the same for all individuals (i.e., no random effects; [81]), while for K > 1 the behavioural dynamics of a given individual are governed by one of K transition probability matrices [27, 28]. It may be possible that > 4 behavioural ‘types’ exist and limiting K to a maximum of 4 was a heuristic choice, aimed at maximizing parsimony (i.e., assuming only a few behavioural types) and computational tractability. Following Isojunno et al. [80], these models were compared using Akaike information criteria (AIC) to select the optimal value for K, where the lowest value is indicative of the best fitting model [83]. Models were fit to data from each interpolation interval (i.e., 5, 10 and 15-min) and selected among based on model pseudo-residuals, which fulfil the role of normal-theory regression residuals for hidden Markov models [73].

The selected interpolation interval and random-effects structure was then used to model the influence of environmental covariates on behavioural transitions. Typically, entries within the transition probability matrix are assumed to be constant, however we relaxed this assumption and estimated the effect of a suite of time-varying environmental covariates on these probabilities (i.e., we assume the Markov chain is non-homogenous; [27]). This was achieved using a multinomial logit-link function which ensures all transition probabilities are between 0 and 1, and the rows of the transition probability matrix sum to 1 [74]. State transition probabilities were modelled as a function of water temperature (°C); an interaction term between tide height (m above Port Stephens Height Datum [PSHD]), and the difference in tide height over 15-min intervals (hereafter ∆-tide height); habitat type; and a cyclic effect of time (hour) of day. Cyclic effects were estimated via two periodic functions, \(\mathrm{cos}(\frac{2\pi t}{24})\) and \(\mathrm{sin}(\frac{2\pi t}{24})\), where t is the time (hour) of day (0–24) and 24 is the assumed daily periodicity of the function [29, 71, 84]. ∆-tide height includes information about both the direction of the tide (positive/negative values = flood/ebb tide) and the strength of tidal currents, where greater absolute values imply stronger tidal currents. Tide data was obtained from a nearby tide gauge (~ 4 km away; 32.72° S, 152.02° E) maintained by Manly Hydraulics Laboratory [85]. Habitat data was obtained from NSW Department of Primary Industries Fisheries Spatial Data Portal (https://www.dpi.nsw.gov.au/about-us/research-development/spatial-data-portal). This dataset includes information on the distribution of common estuarine habitats, including: seagrass, mangroves and saltmarsh [86], with a spatial resolution of approximately ± 2 m (G. West, pers. comm.). To account for edge effects around seagrass meadows [87, 88] a buffer of 1.26 m was applied (matching the median error in our array; see Hyperbolic positioning). All possible combinations of covariates were fit (including ‘null’ models with no covariates), however the tidal covariates were only included together. These models were compared using AIC, where the model with the lowest value was selected as the true model [83]. Stationary state probabilities were derived from the transition probability matrix and can be interpreted as the probability of exhibiting a given state for some fixed value of a covariate (i.e., when the system is in equilibrium). Finally, behavioural states at each location were estimated using the Viterbi algorithm, which derives the most likely sequence of states given the observations and fitted model [25, 73]. Model fit was again assessed by inspecting pseudo-residuals.

Results

Model selection and diagnostics

For all interpolation intervals (5-, 10- and 15-min), we found no evidence of individual-level variation in crab behaviour (i.e., K = 1 mixture had the lowest AIC). Furthermore, AIC increased with increasing K suggesting that it is unlikely that > 4 behavioural ‘types’ exist within the tagged population (Additional file 1: Table S1). Therefore, we modelled the influence of environmental covariates on crab behavioural dynamics using a ‘standard’ hidden Markov model (i.e., without random effects). Model pseudo-residuals indicated that data interpolated at 15-min intervals provided the best fit relative to the 5- and 10-min data (Additional file 1: Fig. S3). On this basis, we report only results from the model fit to data interpolated at 15-min intervals. Furthermore, we only report results from our two-state model, since the three-state model simply decomposed one state (‘inactive’ state, see Crab behavioural states) into two and would not have changed our biological interpretation. Finally, model selection for covariate inclusion indicated that the model including an interaction between tide height and ∆-tide height provided the best fit (Additional file 1: Table S2).

There was evidence of a diel cycle in behaviour not captured by this model, indicated by cyclic residual autocorrelation for both step length and acceleration with a ~ 12 h period (Additional file 1: Fig. S3b, f). However, the model that included a cyclic effect of time of day did not improve this. Ultimately, model fit was deemed adequate since hidden Markov models do not need to produce perfectly independent pseudo-residuals [24, 73] and small violations of this are generally of little concern when estimating behavioural state dynamics is the main goal of analysis [28] as is the case here.

The selected model was fit to 75 tracks from 13 individuals, ranging in size from 119 to 135 mm CL (Table 1). The length of tracks ranged from ~ 8 h–20 d, with an average of 1.5 ± 3.2 d. We found no evidence of tagging effects and only 9 of these 75 tracks included detections from a crab on the same day as tagging (further limiting any possible influence of tagging effects in our analysis). One female crab (ID = 7792) was detected in the coastal ocean (via the IMOS ATF) approximately 150 km north at the Port Macquarie offshore artificial reef (~ 31.42° S) 27 days after the last detection in our array.

Table 1 Biological and detection information for tagged Giant Mud Crab included in modelling

Crab behavioural states

Our analysis identified behavioural states with considerable overlap in terms of their step length and turning angle concentration, however there was clear separation in terms of acceleration (Fig. 2). State 1 is likely to represent foraging (hereafter ‘foraging state’) since crabs spent little time in this state (21%) and exhibited greater, but highly variable, step.lengths (mean ± SD = 13.98 ± 18.10 m 15 min−1) and acceleration (0.59 ± 0.63 m s−2 15 min−1), coupled with moderately concentrated turning angles (concentration = 0.51; Fig. 2; Table 2), indicative of a combination of straight-line movement and direction changes. In the foraging state, crabs also had the lowest probability of no movement (Table 2). However, they exhibited a higher probability of exhibiting no acceleration which is likely due to the relatively high variability in acceleration while in this state (Table 2). Crabs spent most of their time in State 2 (79%), which is likely to correspond to periods of inactivity (hereafter ‘inactive state’). Crabs in this state exhibited much shorter step lengths (mean ± SD = 0.75 ± 0.93 m 15 min−1), low acceleration (0.04 ± 0.01 m s−2 15 min−1), and relatively highly concentrated turning angles (concentration = 0.70), indicative of infrequent changes in direction (Fig. 2; Table 2). An example track, with the most likely sequence of states is depicted in Fig. 3.

Fig. 2
figure 2

State-dependent probability distributions (lines) and histograms of observations (grey bars) for a step length (m), b turning angle (radians) and c) acceleration (m s−2). Note x-axis on (a, c) has been truncated to aid visualization and excludes the upper ~ 3% of observations

Table 2 State-dependent parameter estimates for tagged Giant Mud Crab
Fig. 3
figure 3

a Example track from a tagged female Giant Mud Crab (ID: 7795) and b the time-series of Viterbi-decoded behavioural states and the corresponding probabilities of c State 1 (foraging; yellow) and d State 2 (inactive; purple)

Behavioural state dynamics

States were highly persistent through time, indicated by very high state-dwell probabilities (i.e., diagonal entries in Table 3). Based on our model selection, water temperature (°C), time (hour) of day and habitat type were removed from our model, implying these covariates explain little about crab behavioural dynamics. The interaction between tide height (m) and ∆-tide height (m 15-min−1) suggested that crabs were most likely to be foraging during the low (< 0.5 m) incoming tide (Fig. 4), and more likely to be inactive as the high tide recedes (Fig. 4). Overall, crabs were always more likely to be inactive than foraging (Fig. 5), but they were approximately twice as likely to be foraging at low tide than at high (Fig. 5a). Conversely, crabs were approximately 1.25 times more likely to be inactive at high tide relative to low (Fig. 5a). The probability of foraging was higher during an incoming tide than an outgoing tide (Fig. 5b), while the opposite is true for the inactive state which becomes more likely as the tide recedes (Fig. 5b).

Table 3 State transition probabilities of tagged Giant Mud Crab
Fig. 4
figure 4

Stationary state probabilities as function of the interaction between tide height (m) and ∆-tide height (m 15-min−1) in tagged Giant Mud Crab

Fig. 5
figure 5

Stationary state probabilities (± 95% CI) as a function of a tide height (m) and b ∆-tide height (m 15-min−1)

Discussion

Our study is the first to quantitatively link fine-scale movement and behaviour of Giant Mud Crab to environmental variation. This was achieved using high spatial and temporal resolution, accelerometer-equipped acoustic tags and high temporal resolution environmental data. Modelling data with a hidden Markov model allowed the classification of observations and provided insight into the fine-scale drivers of behaviour for the species. Our analysis shows that the tidal cycle is an important driver of foraging, that allows crabs to minimize visual predation risk, and optimize the energetic efficiency of foraging. Quantifying movement and behaviour of mobile exploited species is important for developing effective fisheries management [2, 3], and our results contribute to the evidence base that underpins management actions for this species.

Fine-scale movement and behaviour of Giant Mud Crab

Direct observation of aquatic animal behaviour is challenging, especially in turbid estuarine waters. The main advantage of hidden Markov models when applied to animal movement is the classification of observed movement and acceleration into ‘states’ that may correspond to biologically meaningful behaviours [23,24,25]. In our analysis, Giant Mud Crab were inactive for a majority of the time (79%), which shows they are a predominantly sessile species [38, 43, 46]. Adult Giant Mud Crab are considered opportunistic scavengers [42], and foraging is facilitated via a combination of olfaction [45] and contact chemoreception [44]. Crabs generally exhibit an initial ‘searching’ response towards olfactory cues [45] to find the approximate location of the food [78], followed by further tactile investigation using the dactyls of the walking legs to find the exact location of the prey/food item [44]. This description of foraging is well explained by the foraging state in our model, which includes a combination of long and short movements with higher overall activity (i.e., acceleration), and variability in terms of directional persistence.

For aquatic species that inhabit shallow-water habitats, the semidiurnal (i.e., twice daily) tidal cycle imposes a regular change in the prevailing conditions [51]. We found that Giant Mud Crab are most likely to forage when the tide is low (< 0.5 m) and incoming, while they are most likely to be inactive on an outgoing, high tide. This likely reflects exploration of shallow or intertidal foraging habitat (e.g., mangroves, mudflats; [49, 77]) as the tide is rising and they become inundated. Larger predators (e.g., Carcharhinus leucas, Bull Shark) are unlikely to be able to access these habitats when water levels are shallower, thereby lowering predation risk—a strategy employed by other estuarine species (e.g., Acanthopagrus australis, Yellowfin Bream; 63). Similarly, the high probability of inactivity during high tide likely reflects a predator avoidance strategy, since Giant Mud Crab typically bury in the mud during periods of inactivity [38, 46]. This description of foraging behaviour closely matches observations of the distribution of Giant Mud Crab in an intertidal region in a nearby estuary (Moreton Bay, Queensland; [77]). Additionally, foraging during strong incoming tides may also be indicative of the use of selective tidal-stream transport [52], a behaviour exhibited by the species elsewhere [39, 41, 53]. If crabs use incoming tides to facilitate movement this may decrease the energetic cost of foraging [52], thereby minimizing the energetic trade-off of searching for prey [89]. However, this may come at the cost of being able to efficiently sample olfactory queues, since swimming with the current is likely to result in a crab remaining in the same olfactory ‘patch’.

In general, warmer temperatures promote foraging in Giant Mud Crab [46]. This is likely due to increased metabolism [90], which may increase their motivation to feed and energetic requirements [6, 91]. However, our analysis suggests that water temperature is not an important driver of fine-scale movement and behaviour of Giant Mud Crab. It is likely that the results presented in Hill [46] represent the seasonal influence of temperature (since they included a period of acclimation) on Giant Mud Crab behaviour, while our analysis is aimed at a much finer temporal resolution (e.g., observations every 15-min over several days) over which the variation of estuarine water temperature is comparatively low. Future studies could employ longer random transmission intervals in tags, thereby preserving battery life and enabling inter-seasonal tracking of individuals. Such data may be amenable to analysis using a ‘hierarchical’ hidden Markov model, which models state processes that operate at different timescales [92, 93], provided the assumed dependence structure is appropriate (see Glennie et al. [67]).

We did not find any evidence of nocturnal foraging, which is thought to represent a visual-predator avoidance strategy and has previously been reported for the species [38, 43]. However, in highly turbid waters visual predation is somewhat reduced and overall predation pressure is likely to be much more diffuse. This may explain the lack of diel rhythm in Giant Mud Crab behaviour in the present study, and elsewhere (e.g., [94]). Similarly, our analysis suggests that habitat type has little influence on crab behavioural dynamics, reflecting their status as opportunistic scavengers [42,43,44]. This is further supported by several stable isotope studies that saltmarsh grass (i.e., Sporobolus virginicus; [48, 50]), seagrass [47] and mangroves [49] all contribute to Giant Mud Crab nutrition. Ultimately, stable isotopes provide an indication as to which habitats form the base of an animals diet, and it is likely that crabs tagged in the present study are carnivorous; feeding on benthic macroinvertebrates (e.g., gastropods, crustaceans and molluscs; 43) that are primary consumers across these habitats.

Implications for fisheries management

Quantifying drivers of animal movement is important for effective fisheries assessment, and management that relies on it [2, 3]. For example, catch-per-unit-effort data is assumed to represent an index of relative abundance, and forms the basis of most contemporary stock assessments [95]. However, use of catch-per-unit-effort as an index of abundance assumes that catchability of target individuals is constant [4, 5], and it is important to consider how environmental variation (e.g., low temperatures) influences foraging and responding to baited traps [6], when standardizing and interpreting catch rates [5]. Our results suggest that the tidal cycle is closely related to patterns in foraging, which could influence catchability, making it an important covariate to consider for catch rate standardisation, as is the case for the closely related Blue Swimmer Crab (Portunus armatus; [96]). However, crab fishers may deploy traps for several days at a time, which may lead to an apparent decoupling of the relationship between catchability in the tidal cycle in catch data. While our analysis suggests water temperature does not affect fine-scale behaviours, many studies have shown a strong temperature effect on Giant Mud Crab catch rates [32, 97] and it is likely that this is still an important covariate to include in catch rate standardization.

Several fisheries management strategies require information about the partitioning of time and behaviours among habitats. For example, stocking of hatchery-reared individuals requires that release locations support the suite of habitats required to support routine behaviours (e.g., foraging). Our analysis suggests that Giant Mud Crab may be highly adaptable in this regard, since they did not exhibit a clear preference for foraging in a particular habitat. However, overall productivity of the system must also be considered (e.g., [90, 98]), and specific habitats (e.g., seagrass) may confer other benefits (e.g., enhanced survival of juveniles) that are not considered in our analysis [99].

Technical considerations and caveats

Tag loss is an important concern in acoustic tagging studies [100, 101], especially when externally tagging crustaceans, as the exoskeleton will be shed during ecdysis [58]. In our study, this was avoided by only tagging large, recently moulted individuals (see Crab capture and tagging) and our approach was largely successful, resulting in only two tag loss incidents. These were likely a consequence of re-entering a trap rather than ecdysis, as glue was present on the carapace of these individuals and they were still in ‘hard-shell’ condition [57] when recaptured. Ultimately, the number of tagged crabs was within the range appropriate for making behavioural inferences at a population scale [101].

Recently, ‘tagging effects’ (e.g., elevated activity) have been documented in other crab species (e.g., Snow Crab, Chionoecetes opilio), which is typically dealt with by discarding detections from the first day of tracking [102, 103]. While we found no evidence of changes in activity, tagging effects in crabs may manifest themselves in ways not amenable to visual inspection of activity, such as burial (i.e., seeking refuge) or emigration from the area. However, our analysis requires relatively long series of consecutive detections (≥ 100), permitting only small interruptions (≤ 1 h) otherwise the data is excluded, and it is likely burial, or emigration would violate these conditions. Furthermore, catch-and-release does not typically induce high levels of stress in the species [34] and previous tagging studies have not found evidence of tagging effects [38, 39] giving us confidence that they have not influenced the analysis presented here.

The importance of accounting for individual-level variation in behaviour is increasingly being recognized in animal tracking studies [79]. This variation can be due to true differences (i.e., animal ‘personality’; [79]), variable deployment lengths and different (unmeasured) environmental contexts encountered by tagged individuals [28]. In our analysis, we found no evidence of individual-level variation in fine-scale behavioural dynamics of Giant Mud Crab. This may be because we only tagged large, adult crabs (119–135 mm CL) that had recently moulted or because our sample size was low relative to the frequency that individual variation is exhibited within the population. It is possible to modify this approach to account for sex-specific differences in behaviour (e.g., [29]), however this approach typically requires larger sample sizes to be reliable [81] and previous studies have not detected any differences in fine-scale behaviour between sexes [46]. Conversely, at greater temporal scales (e.g., seasonal) differences in movement have been observed. For example, mature female Giant Mud Crab typically migrate to oceanic waters to spawn [39, 40, 53], facilitating the broad-scale dispersal of larvae [104] which may explain the detection of a tagged female ~ 150 km north of our array in the coastal ocean. While males are typically thought to remain within estuaries, there have been a few examples of broad-scale migrations reported [41].

Measurement error within our array was generally low (median = 1.26 m) and consistent with inherent GPS error (2–3 m; [17]), which was used to define the ‘known’ positions of receivers and reference tags. Additionally, some error may have been due to the presence of structurally complex habitats such as seagrass, mangroves and oyster farming infrastructure, which can lead to issues with signal attenuation [60], refraction, or reflection (sometimes refered to as ‘multipath’; [105]). Measurement error can increase the overlap between state-dependent distributions (e.g., similar step lengths) which may lead to difficulty differentiating states [106]. The inclusion of acceleration—which is not subject to any location measurement error—buffers against this in our analysis, and acceleration within the foraging state was markedly different from the inactive state. Despite this, some state misclassification may have still occurred. For example, we would expect crabs in the inactive state to have relatively low concentration in their turning angles, given consecutive location estimates around the true location. However, the relatively high concentration in this state may imply that some very fine-scale (i.e., < 1 m), low-acceleration foraging movements (e.g., scavenging on a fish carcass) were assigned to the inactive state, or that resting crabs may drift in currents to some extent. State classification can be further aided by fitting hidden Markov models in a semi-supervised context via the incorporation of ‘known’ (or labelled) states, which are typically derived from laboratory observations [24]. However, movements in a laboratory setting may not be representative of free-ranging animals [24, 107] and incorporation of labelled data can be practically and computationally challenging (V. Leos-Barajas, pers. comm.).

Finally, acoustic tags must be submerged to record detections, and while it is possible for Giant Mud Crab to spend prolonged periods out of the water, this is uncommon for adults in the size range tagged [77] giving us confidence that this did not exert undue influence on the results presented here.

Conclusions

Our description of Giant Mud Crab behavioural dynamics are in close agreement with observations of Giant Mud Crab behaviour [43,44,45,46] and qualitatively similar to a previous active-tracking study (that did not record/report any environmental data; [38]). Furthermore, these results provide a mechanistic explanation of the observed distribution of the species across sub–intertidal habitats [77]. We demonstrate the importance of the tidal cycle in driving foraging of Giant Mud Crab, likely as a strategy to minimize predation and maximize energetic efficiency, similar to other estuarine species [63]. Determining such relationships adds to the evidence base supporting fisheries management [2, 3] and the patterns resolve aid the standardisation and interpretation of catch-per-unit-effort data.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated and/or analyzed during the current study can be accessed through the IMOS ATF (https://animaltracking.aodn.org.au) under the NSW DPI Coastal and Estuarine Fish Tracking (CEFT) project. The code used for analysis can be accessed via GitHub (https://github.com/DEHewitt/gmc_vps_hmm) or upon reasonable request to the corresponding author. The hyperbolic positioning algorithms used are the property of Innovasea and are unavailable at the time of publication.

Abbreviations

CL:

Carapace length

ODBA:

Overall dynamic body acceleration

RMS:

Root mean square

SD:

Standard deviation

TDOA:

Time-difference-of-arrival

∆-tide height:

Difference in tide height in 15-min intervals

References

  1. McClintock BT, Abrahms B, Chandler RB, Conn PB, Converse SJ, Emmet RL, et al. An integrated path for spatial capture–recapture and animal movement modeling. Ecology. 2022;103(10):e3473. https://doi.org/10.1002/ecy.3473.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Cooke SJ, Martins EG, Struthers DP, Gutowsky LF, Power M, Doka SE, et al. A moving target—incorporating knowledge of the spatial ecology of fish into the assessment and management of freshwater fish populations. Environ Monit Assess. 2016;188(4):239. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10661-016-5228-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Taylor MD, Babcock RC, Simpfendorfer CA, Crook DA. Where technology meets ecology: acoustic telemetry in contemporary Australian aquatic research and management. Mar Freshw Res. 2017;68(8):1397–402. https://doi.org/10.1071/Mf17054.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Maunder MN, Punt AE. Standardizing catch and effort data: a review of recent approaches. Fish Res. 2004;70(2–3):141–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.fishres.2004.08.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Maunder MN, Sibert JR, Fonteneau A, Hampton J, Kleiber P, Harley SJ. Interpreting catch per unit effort data to assess the status of individual stocks and communities. ICES J Mar Sci. 2006;63(8):1373–85. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.icesjms.2006.05.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Stoner AW. Effects of environmental variables on fish feeding ecology: implications for the performance of baited fishing gear and stock assessment. J Fish Biol. 2004;65(6):1445–71. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0022-1112.2004.00593.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Payne NL, van der Meulen DE, Suthers IM, Gray CA, Taylor MD. Foraging intensity of wild mulloway Argyrosomus japonicus decreases with increasing anthropogenic disturbance. Mar Biol. 2015;162(3):539–46. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00227-014-2603-7.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Puckeridge AC, Becker A, Taylor MD, Lowry MB, McLeod J, Schilling HT, et al. Foraging behaviour and movements of an ambush predator reveal benthopelagic coupling on artificial reefs. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2021;666:171–82. https://doi.org/10.3354/meps13691.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Stehfest KM, Lyle JM, Semmens JM. The use of acoustic accelerometer tags to determine seasonal changes in activity and catchability of a recreationally caught marine teleost. ICES J Mar Sci. 2015;72(8):2512–20. https://doi.org/10.1093/icesjms/fsv115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Ferguson AM, Harvey ES, Taylor MD, Knott NA. A herbivore knows its patch: Luderick, Girella tricuspidata, exhibit strong site fidelity on shallow subtidal reefs in a temperate marine park. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(5):e65838. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0065838.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Fetterplace LC, Davis AR, Neilson JM, Taylor MD, Knott NA. Active acoustic tracking suggests that soft sediment fishes can show site attachment: a preliminary assessment of the movement patterns of the blue-spotted flathead (Platycephalus caeruleopunctatus). Anim Biotelemetry. 2016;4(1):15. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40317-016-0107-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Taylor MD, Creighton C. Estimating the potential fishery benefits from targeted habitat repair: a case study of school prawn (Metapenaeus macleayi) in the Lower Clarence River Estuary. Wetlands. 2018;38(6):1199–209. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13157-018-1022-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Bennett MA, Becker A, Gaston T, Taylor MD. Connectivity of large-bodied fish with a recovering estuarine tidal marsh, revealed using an imaging sonar. Estuar Coasts. 2020;44(6):1–9. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12237-020-00822-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Crossin GT, Heupel MR, Holbrook CM, Hussey NE, Lowerre-Barbieri SK, Nguyen VM, et al. Acoustic telemetry and fisheries management. Ecol Appl. 2017;27(4):1031–49. https://doi.org/10.1002/eap.1533.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Espinoza M, Farrugia TJ, Webber DM, Smith F, Lowe CG. Testing a new acoustic telemetry technique to quantify long-term, fine-scale movements of aquatic animals. Fish Res. 2011;108(2–3):364–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.fishres.2011.01.011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Roy R, Beguin J, Argillier C, Tissot L, Smith F, Smedbol S, et al. Testing the VEMCO Positioning System: spatial distribution of the probability of location and the positioning error in a reservoir. Anim Biotelemetry. 2014;2(1):1. https://doi.org/10.1186/2050-3385-2-1.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Smith F. Understanding HPE in the VEMCO positioning system (VPS). 2013.

  18. Nathan R, Spiegel O, Fortmann-Roe S, Harel R, Wikelski M, Getz WM. Using tri-axial acceleration data to identify behavioral modes of free-ranging animals: general concepts and tools illustrated for griffon vultures. J Exp Biol. 2012;215(6):986–96. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.058602.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Payne NL, Taylor MD, Watanabe YY, Semmens JM. From physiology to physics: are we recognizing the flexibility of biologging tools? J Exp Biol. 2014;217(3):317–22. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.093922.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Hooten MB, Johnson DS, McClintock BT, Morales JM. Animal movement: statistical models for telemetry data. CRC Press; 2017.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  21. Patterson TA, Parton A, Langrock R, Blackwell PG, Thomas L, King R. Statistical modelling of individual animal movement: an overview of key methods and a discussion of practical challenges. AStA Adv Stat Anal. 2017;101(4):399–438. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10182-017-0302-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Joo R, Boone ME, Clay TA, Patrick SC, Clusella-Trullas S, Basille M. Navigating through the R packages for movement. J Anim Ecol. 2020;89(1):248–67. https://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2656.13116.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Langrock R, King R, Matthiopoulos J, Thomas L, Fortin D, Morales JM. Flexible and practical modeling of animal telemetry data: hidden Markov models and extensions. Ecol. 2012;93(11):2336–42. https://doi.org/10.1890/11-2241.1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Leos-Barajas V, Photopoulou T, Langrock R, Patterson TA, Watanabe YY, Murgatroyd M, et al. Analysis of animal accelerometer data using hidden Markov models. Methods Ecol Evol. 2017;8(2):161–73. https://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210X.12657.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. McClintock BT, Langrock R, Gimenez O, Cam E, Borchers DL, Glennie R, et al. Uncovering ecological state dynamics with hidden Markov models. Ecol Lett. 2020;23(12):1878–903. https://doi.org/10.1111/ele.13610.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Patterson TA, Basson M, Bravington MV, Gunn JS. Classifying movement behaviour in relation to environmental conditions using hidden Markov models. J Anim Ecol. 2009;78(6):1113–23. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2656.2009.01583.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. McKellar AE, Langrock R, Walters JR, Kesler DC. Using mixed hidden Markov models to examine behavioral states in a cooperatively breeding bird. Behav Ecol. 2014;26(1):148–57. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/aru171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. DeRuiter S, Langrock R, Skirbutas T, Goldbogen J, Calambokidis J, Friedlaender A, et al. A multivariate mixed hidden Markov model for blue whale behaviour and responses to sound exposure. Ann Appl Stat. 2017;11:362–92. https://doi.org/10.1214/16-AOAS1008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Towner AV, Leos-Barajas V, Langrock R, Schick RS, Smale MJ, Kaschke T, et al. Sex-specific and individual preferences for hunting strategies in white sharks. Funct Ecol. 2016;30(8):1397–407. https://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2435.12613.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Keenan C, Davie PJ, Mann D. A revision of the genus Scylla de Haan, 1833 (Crustacea: Decapoda: Brachyura: Portunidae). Raffles Bull Zoo. 1998;46:217–45.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Portunidae DP. Crustacea: Malacostraca: Eucarida (Part 2), Decapoda: Anomura. Brachyura: CSIRO Publishing; 2002.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Meynecke J, Grubert M, Gillson J. Giant mud crab (Scylla serrata) catches and climate drivers in Australia–a large scale comparison. Mar Freshw Res. 2012;63(1):84–94. https://doi.org/10.1071/Mf11149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. West LD, Stark KE, Murphy JJ, Lyle JM, Ochwada-Doyle FA. Survey of recreational fishing in New South Wales and the ACT, 2013/14. NSW Department of Primary Industries, Nelson Bay; 2016.

  34. Butcher PA, Leland JC, Broadhurst MK, Paterson BD, Mayer DG. Giant mud crab (Scylla serrata): relative efficiencies of common baited traps and impacts on discards. ICES J Mar Sci. 2012;69(8):1511–22. https://doi.org/10.1093/icesjms/fss109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Barnes TC, Broadhurst MK, Johnson DD. Disparity among recommended and adopted escape-gap designs and their utility for improving selection in an Australian portunid trap fishery. Fish Res. 2022;248:106219. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.fishres.2021.106219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Hyland S, Hill B, Lee C. Movement within and between different habitats by the portunid crab Scylla serrata. Mar Biol. 1984;80(1):57–61. https://doi.org/10.1007/Bf00393128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Alberts-Hubatsch H, Lee SY, Diele K, Wolff M, Nordhaus I. Microhabitat use of early benthic stage mud crabs, Scylla serrata (Forskal, 1775), in eastern Australia. J Crust Biol. 2014;34(5):604–10. https://doi.org/10.1163/1937240x-00002256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Hill B. Activity, track and speed of movement of the crab Scylla serrata in an estuary. Mar Biol. 1978;47(2):135–41. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00395634.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Hewitt DE, Niella Y, Johnson DD, Suthers IM, Taylor MD. Crabs go with the flow: declining conductivity and cooler temperatures trigger spawning migrations for female Giant Mud Crabs (Scylla serrata) in subtropical estuaries. Estuar Coasts. 2022;1:2. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12237-022-01061-1.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. Hill B. Offshore spawning by the portunid crab Scylla serrata (Crustacea: Decapoda). Mar Biol. 1994;120(3):379–84. https://doi.org/10.1007/Bf00680211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Patterson RG, Wolanski E, Groom R, Critchell K, Playford L, Grubert M, et al. Improving certainty in marine ecosystems: a biophysical modelling approach in the remote, data-limited Gulf of Carpentaria. Estuar Coast Shelf Sci. 2023. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecss.2023.108254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Webley JA. The ecology of the mud crab (Scylla serrata): their colonisation of estuaries and role as scavengers in ecosystem processes. Queensland: Griffith University; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Hill BJ. Natural food, foregut clearance-rate and activity of the crab Scylla serrata. Mar Biol. 1976;34(2):109–16. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00390752.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Hill BJ. Aspects of the feeding strategy of the predatory crab Scylla serrata. Mar Biol. 1979;55(3):209–14. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00396820.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Wall D, Paterson B, Mohan R. Behaviour of juvenile mud crabs Scylla serrata in aquaculture: response to odours of moulting or injured crabs. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2009;121:63–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2009.08.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Hill B. Effects of temperature on feeding and activity in the crab Scylla serrata. Mar Biol. 1980;59(3):189–92. https://doi.org/10.1007/Bf00396867.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Connolly RM, Waltham NJ. Spatial analysis of carbon isotopes reveals seagrass contribution to fishery food web. Ecosphere. 2015;6(9):art148. https://doi.org/10.1890/es14-00243.1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Jänes H, Macreadie PI, Rizzari J, Ierodioconou D, Reeves SE, Dwyer PG, et al. The value of estuarine producers to fisheries: a case study of Richmond River Estuary. Ambio. 2022;51(4):875–87. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13280-021-01600-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Demopoulos AWJ, Cormier N, Ewel KC, Fry B. Use of multiple chemical tracers to define habitat use of Indo-Pacific mangrove crab, Scylla serrata (Decapoda: Portunidae). Estuar Coasts. 2008;31(2):371–81. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12237-007-9008-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Raoult V, Gaston TF, Taylor MD. Habitat–fishery linkages in two major south-eastern Australian estuaries show that the C4 saltmarsh plant Sporobolus virginicus is a significant contributor to fisheries productivity. Hydrobiologia. 2018;811(1):221–38. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10750-017-3490-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Gibson RN. Go with the flow: tidal migration in marine animals. Hydrobiologia. 2003;503(1):153–61. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:HYDR.0000008488.33614.62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Forward R, Tankersley R. Selective tidal-stream transport of marine animals. Oceanogr Mar Biol. 2001;39:305–53.

    Google Scholar 

  53. Alberts-Hubatsch H. Movement patterns and habitat use of the exploited swimming crab Scylla serrata (Forskål, 1775). Ph.D. thesis: University Bremen, Germany; 2015.

  54. Roy PS, Williams RJ, Jones AR, Yassini I, Gibbs PJ, Coates B, et al. Structure and function of south-east Australian estuaries. Estuar Coast Shelf Sci. 2001;53(3):351–84. https://doi.org/10.1006/ecss.2001.0796.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Walsh CT, Reinfelds IV, West RJ, Gray CA, van der Meulen DE. Distribution and movement of catadromous fish: design and implementation of a freshwater-estuarine acoustic telemetry array. Am Fish Soc Symp. 2012;76:251–64.

    Google Scholar 

  56. Taylor MD, Payne NL, Becker A, Lowry MB. Feels like home: homing of mature large-bodied fish following translocation from a power-station canal. ICES J Mar Sci. 2016;74(1):301–10. https://doi.org/10.1093/icesjms/fsw168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Hay T, Gribble N, de Vries C, Danaher K, Dunning M, Hearnden M, et al. Methods for monitoring the abundance and habitat of the northern Australian mud crab Scylla serrata. Northern Territory Department of Business, Industry and Resource Development, Darwin; 2005.

  58. Florko KRN, Davidson ER, Lees KJ, Hammer LJ, Lavoie MF, Lennox RJ, et al. Tracking movements of decapod crustaceans: a review of a half-century of telemetry-based studies. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2021;679:219–39. https://doi.org/10.3354/meps13904.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Grothues TM, Able KW, Pravatiner JH. Winter flounder (Pseudopleuronectes americanus Walbaum) burial in estuaries: acoustic telemetry triumph and tribulation. J Exp Mar Biol Ecol. 2012;438:125–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jembe.2012.09.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Swadling DS, Knott NA, Rees MJ, Pederson H, Adams KR, Taylor MD, et al. Seagrass canopies and the performance of acoustic telemetry: implications for the interpretation of fish movements. Anim Biotelemetry. 2020;8(1):1–12. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40317-020-00197-w.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Wilson RP, White CR, Quintana F, Halsey LG, Liebsch N, Martin GR, et al. Moving towards acceleration for estimates of activity-specific metabolic rate in free-living animals: the case of the cormorant. J Anim Ecol. 2006;75(5):1081–90. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2656.2006.01127.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Qasem L, Cardew A, Wilson A, Griffiths I, Halsey LG, Shepard EL, et al. Tri-axial dynamic acceleration as a proxy for animal energy expenditure; should we be summing values or calculating the vector? PLoS ONE. 2012;7(2):e31187. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0031187.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. Taylor MD, McPhan L, van der Meulen DE, Gray CA, Payne NL. Interactive drivers of activity in a free-ranging estuarine predator. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(11):e80962. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080962.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Coppens AB. Simple equations for the speed of sound in Neptunian waters. J Acoust Soc Am. 1981;69(3):862–3. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.385486.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. R Development Core Team. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2019.

    Google Scholar 

  66. McClintock BT, Johnson DS, Hooten MB, Ver Hoef JM, Morales JM. When to be discrete: the importance of time formulation in understanding animal movement. Mov Ecol. 2014;2(1):1–14. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40462-014-0021-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Glennie R, Adam T, Leos-Barajas V, Michelot T, Photopoulou T, McClintock BT. Hidden Markov models: pitfalls and opportunities in ecology. Methods Ecol Evol. 2022;1:2. https://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210X.13801.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Johnson DS, London JM. crawl: an R package for fitting continuous-time correlated random walk models to animal movement data. Zenodo; 2018.

  69. Johnson DS, London JM, Lea M-A, Durban JW. Continuous-time correlated random walk model for animal telemetry data. Ecology. 2008;89(5):1208–15. https://doi.org/10.1890/07-1032.1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Gurarie E, Bracis C, Delgado M, Meckley TD, Kojola I, Wagner CM. What is the animal doing? Tools for exploring behavioural structure in animal movements. J Anim Ecol. 2016;85(1):69–84. https://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2656.12379.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Bacheler NM, Michelot T, Cheshire RT, Shertzer KW. Fine-scale movement patterns and behavioral states of gray triggerfish Balistes capriscus determined from acoustic telemetry and hidden Markov models. Fish Res. 2019;215:76–89. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.fishres.2019.02.014.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. McClintock BT, Michelot T. momentuHMM: R package for generalized hidden Markov models of animal movement. Methods Ecol Evol. 2018;9(6):1518–30. https://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210x.12995.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Zucchini W, MacDonald IL, Langrock R. Hidden Markov models for time series: an introduction using R. CRC Press; 2017.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  74. Michelot T, Langrock R, Patterson TA. moveHMM: an R package for the statistical modelling of animal movement data using hidden Markov models. Methods Ecol Evol. 2016;7(11):1308–15. https://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210x.12578.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Li M, Bolker BM. Incorporating periodic variability in hidden Markov models for animal movement. Mov Ecol. 2017;5(1):1. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40462-016-0093-6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  76. Pohle J, Langrock R, van Beest FM, Schmidt NM. Selecting the number of states in hidden Markov models: pragmatic solutions illustrated using animal movement. J Agric Biol Environ Stat. 2017;22(3):270–93. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13253-017-0283-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Hill B, Williams M, Dutton P. Distribution of juvenile, subadult and adult Scylla serrata (Crustacea: Portunidae) on tidal flats in Australia. Mar Biol. 1982;69(1):117–20. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00396967.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Alberts-Hubatsch H, Lee SY, Meynecke J-O, Diele K, Nordhaus I, Wolff M. Life-history, movement, and habitat use of Scylla serrata (Decapoda, Portunidae): current knowledge and future challenges. Hydrobiologia. 2016;763(1):5–21. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10750-015-2393-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Hertel AG, Niemelä PT, Dingemanse NJ, Mueller T. A guide for studying among-individual behavioral variation from movement data in the wild. Mov Ecol. 2020;8(1):30. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40462-020-00216-8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  80. Isojunno S, Sadykova D, DeRuiter S, Curé C, Visser F, Thomas L, et al. Individual, ecological, and anthropogenic influences on activity budgets of long-finned pilot whales. Ecosphere. 2017;8(12):e02044. https://doi.org/10.1002/ecs2.2044.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  81. McClintock BT. Worth the effort? A practical examination of random effects in hidden Markov models for animal telemetry data. Methods Ecol Evol. 2021;12(8):1475–97. https://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210X.13619.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  82. McClintock BT, Michelot T. momentuHMM: R package for analysis of telemetry data using generalized multivariate hidden Markov models of animal movement. 2017.

  83. Burnham KP, Anderson DR. Model selection and multimodel inference: a practical information-theoretic approach. 2nd ed. New York: Springer; 2002.

    Google Scholar 

  84. Cornelissen G. Cosinor-based rhythmometry. Theor Biol Med Model. 2014;11(1):16. https://doi.org/10.1186/1742-4682-11-16.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  85. NSW DPIE. Mallabula Point tide gauge. Manly Hydraulics Laboratory, New South Wales Department of Planning, Industry and Environment; 2021.

  86. Creese RG, Glasby TM, Gallen C. Mapping the habitats of NSW estuaries. Port Stephens: Industry & Investment NSW; 2009.

    Google Scholar 

  87. Smith TM, Hindell JS, Jenkins GP, Connolly RM. Edge effects on fish associated with seagrass and sand patches. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2008;359:203–13. https://doi.org/10.3354/meps07348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  88. Smith TM, Hindell JS, Jenkins GP, Connolly RM, Keough MJ. Edge effects in patchy seagrass landscapes: the role of predation in determining fish distribution. J Exp Mar Biol Ecol. 2011;399(1):8–16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jembe.2011.01.010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  89. Kramer DL, Weary DM. Exploration versus exploitation: a field study of time allocation to environmental tracking by foraging chipmunks. Anim Behav. 1991;41(3):443–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  90. Junk EJ, Smith JA, Suthers IM, Taylor MD. Bioenergetics of blue swimmer crab (Portunus armatus) to inform estimation of release density for stock enhancement. Mar Freshw Res. 2021;72(9):1375–86. https://doi.org/10.1071/MF20363.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  91. Green BS, Gardner C, Hochmuth JD, Linnane A. Environmental effects on fished lobsters and crabs. Rev Fish Biol Fish. 2014;24(2):613–38. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11160-014-9350-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  92. Leos-Barajas V, Gangloff EJ, Adam T, Langrock R, van Beest FM, Nabe-Nielsen J, et al. Multi-scale modeling of animal movement and general behavior data using hidden Markov models with hierarchical structures. J Agric Biol Environ Stat. 2017;22(3):232–48. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13253-017-0282-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  93. Adam T, Griffiths CA, Leos-Barajas V, Meese EN, Lowe CG, Blackwell PG, et al. Joint modelling of multi-scale animal movement data using hierarchical hidden Markov models. Methods Ecol Evol. 2019;10(9):1536–50. https://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210X.13241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  94. Robertson WD. Factors affecting catches of the crab Scylla serrata (Forskål) (Decapoda: Portunidae) in baited traps: Soak time, time of day and accessibility of the bait. Estuar Coast Shelf Sci. 1989;29(2):161–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/0272-7714(89)90005-X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  95. Quinn TJ, Deriso RB. Quantitative fish dynamics. Oxford University Press; 1999.

    Google Scholar 

  96. Johnston DJ, Yeoh DE, Harris DC. Environmental drivers of commercial blue swimmer crab (Portunus armatus) catch rates in Western Australian fisheries. Fish Res. 2021;235:105827. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.fishres.2020.105827.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  97. Williams M, Hill B. Factors influencing pot catches and population estimates of the portunid crab Scylla serrata. Mar Biol. 1982;71(2):187–92. https://doi.org/10.1007/Bf00394628.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  98. Smith JA, Eyre BD, Rosentreter JA, Taylor MD. Modelling estuarine stocking density for crustaceans using net ecosystem metabolism. Bull Mar Sci. 2019;95(2):217–38. https://doi.org/10.5343/bms.2018.0047.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  99. Hayes CT, Alford SB, Belgrad BA, Correia KM, Darnell MZ, Furman BT, et al. Regional variation in seagrass complexity drives blue crab Callinectes sapidus mortality and growth across the northern Gulf of Mexico. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2022;693:141–55. https://doi.org/10.3354/meps14084.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  100. Brownscombe JW, Lédée EJI, Raby GD, Struthers DP, Gutowsky LFG, Nguyen VM, et al. Conducting and interpreting fish telemetry studies: considerations for researchers and resource managers. Rev Fish Biol Fish. 2019;29(2):369–400. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11160-019-09560-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  101. Sequeira AMM, Heupel MR, Lea M-A, Eguíluz VM, Duarte CM, Meekan MG, et al. The importance of sample size in marine megafauna tagging studies. Ecol Appl. 2019;29(6):e01947. https://doi.org/10.1002/eap.1947.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  102. Cote D, Morris CJ, Regular PM, Piersiak MG. Effects of 2D seismic on snow crab movement behavior. Fish Res. 2020;230:105661. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.fishres.2020.105661.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  103. Cote D, Nicolas J-M, Whoriskey F, Cook AM, Broome J, Regular PM, et al. Characterizing snow crab (Chionoecetes opilio) movements in the Sydney Bight (Nova Scotia, Canada): a collaborative approach using multiscale acoustic telemetry. Can J Fish Aquat Sci. 2019;76(2):334–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  104. Hewitt DE, Schilling HT, Hanamseth R, Everett JD, Li J, Roughan M, et al. Mesoscale oceanographic features drive divergent patterns in connectivity for co-occurring estuarine portunid crabs. Fish Oceanogr. 2022;31(6):587–600. https://doi.org/10.1111/fog.12608.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  105. Vergeynst J, Vanwyck T, Baeyens R, De Mulder T, Nopens I, Mouton A, et al. Acoustic positioning in a reflective environment: going beyond point-by-point algorithms. Anim Biotelemetry. 2020;8(1):16. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40317-020-00203-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  106. Jonsen I. Joint estimation over multiple individuals improves behavioural state inference from animal movement data. Sci Rep. 2016;6(1):20625. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep20625.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  107. Morgan R, Andreassen AH, Åsheim ER, Finnøen MH, Dresler G, Brembu T, et al. Reduced physiological plasticity in a fish adapted to stable temperatures. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2022;119(22):e2201919119. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2201919119.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to acknowledge B. Leach, A. Wiltshire, A. Becker, J. McLeod, H. Pederson, F. Jaine, K. Whoriskey and V. Leos-Barajas for their assistance with fieldwork, technical or statistical advice. Data for this project was stored on the IMOS ATF (https://animaltracking.aodn.org.au) – IMOS is enabled by the National Collaborative Research Infrastructure Strategy (NCRIS). It is operated by a consortium of institutions as an unincorporated joint venture, with the University of Tasmania as Lead Agent. This is contribution #306 of the Sydney Institute of Marine Science.

Funding

This project was supported by the Fisheries Research and Development Corporation (FRDC) on behalf of the Australian Government through a grant to MDT, DDJ and IMS (2017/006), and co-funded by the NSW Recreational Fishing Saltwater Trust. DEH was supported by an Australian Government Research Training Program (RTP) Scholarship and a NSW DPIE top-up scholarship. Funding bodies and project partners had no role in the design, data collection, analysis or interpretation of data.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

MDT, DDJ and IMS attained funding for this project. DEH, MDT, DDJ and IMS planned the study. DEH conducted the fieldwork, analysis and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. DEH, DDJ, IMS and MDT contributed critically to the drafts and gave final approval for publication. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Daniel E. Hewitt.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Sample collection was conducted under a Section 37 Scientific Collection Permit (permit P01/0059), and Animal Research Authority 13-08 issued by NSW Department of Primary Industries. Consent to participate is not applicable for this study as no humans were research subjects.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no known competing financial or personal interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1. Supplementary Figure 1.

Daily horizontal position error (m) of a fixed-position reference tag within our array. Horizontal position error is the difference between the known position of the tag and its estimated position. The y axis in a) is truncated to show the majority of the data (i.e., 95 % of detections < 10 m horizontal position error), while b) shows all detections.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hewitt, D.E., Johnson, D.D., Suthers, I.M. et al. Crabs ride the tide: incoming tides promote foraging of Giant Mud Crab (Scylla serrata). Mov Ecol 11, 21 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40462-023-00384-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40462-023-00384-3

Keywords