Skip to main content

Movement and habitat selection of a large carnivore in response to human infrastructure differs by life stage

Abstract

Background

The movement extent of mammals is influenced by human-modified areas, which can affect population demographics. Understanding how human infrastructure influences movement at different life stages is important for wildlife management. This is true especially for large carnivores, due to their substantial space requirements and potential for conflict with humans.

Methods

We investigated human impact on movement and habitat selection by GPS-collared male brown bears (Ursus arctos) in two life stages (residents and dispersers) in central Sweden. We identified dispersers visually based on their GPS locations and used hidden Markov models to delineate dispersal events. We used integrated step selection analysis (iSSA) to infer movement and habitat selection at a local scale (availability defined by hourly relocations), and resource selection functions (RSFs) to infer habitat selection at a landscape scale (availability defined by the study area extent).

Results

Movement of residents on a local scale was facilitated by small forestry roads as they moved faster and selected areas closer to forestry roads, and they avoided areas closer to larger public roads and buildings on both scales. Dispersers were more ambivalent in their response to human infrastructure. Dispersers increased their speed closer to small forestry roads and larger public roads, did not exhibit selection for or against any road class, and avoided areas closer to buildings only at local scale. Dispersers did not select for any features on the landscape, which is likely explained by the novelty of the landscape or their naivety towards it.

Conclusion

Our results show that movement in male brown bears is life stage-dependent and indicate that connectivity maps derived from movement data of dispersing animals may provide more numerous and more realistic pathways than those derived from resident animal data alone. This suggests that data from dispersing animals provide more realistic models for reconnecting populations and maintaining connectivity than if data were derived from resident animals alone.

Introduction

Human activity and infrastructure have reduced the movement extent of wildlife globally [1]. Animals are generally sensitive to human infrastructure [2], especially to the creation of linear structures [3], which is commonly reflected in their movement patterns. Maintaining connectivity, i.e. the ease of movement between suitable habitat patches or between populations, within a human-dominated landscape is important to avoid fragmentation of populations and to ensure gene flow [4, 5]. As dispersal contributes to population connectivity and genetic diversity [6], it is crucial to understand how dispersing individuals (“dispersers”) respond to human infrastructure and if they respond differently compared with individuals settled within a home range (“residents”). Connectivity is often derived from habitat selection estimates [4]. Habitat selection can vary across life stages [7, 8], i.e. dispersers compared with residents. Thus, connectivity estimates may differ depending on which life stage the habitat selection estimates are obtained from [8].

Understanding how movement decisions differ by life stage is important for the conservation of species, e.g., for defining potential connectivity and conservation corridors within and between populations [9]. Dispersal can be risky and energetically costly, as it often exposes individuals to unknown environments, especially in human-modified landscapes [10,11,12] where mortality risk can be higher [13]. This implies that dispersers are either unable to perceive human risk or fail to adjust habitat use or movement in response to human risk due to their naivety [14] or they might be more ‘tolerant’ or ‘bold’ and traverse risky habitats [15]. In contrast, the home range is familiar to a resident, and risk encountered during movement may be mitigated through spatiotemporal shifts or altered habitat selection based on prior experience [16, 17]. This strategy might not be available to naïve dispersers facing unexpected or less predictable, risky features on the human-dominated landscape [18].

Habitat selection, i.e. the disproportionate use of a habitat feature in relation to its availability [19], can be estimated at different spatial scales depending on how availability is defined. Throughout this article we refer to habitat selection on the “landscape scale” when the availability is defined for an area many times the size of an animal’s home range, e.g. an entire study area (cf. second order habitat selection [19]). We refer to habitat selection at the “local scale” when the availability is defined over smaller areas or shorter distances (e.g. for step selection functions) that an animal is able to traverse between successive (e.g. hourly) locations (cf. forth order habitat selection [19]). Animals may respond differently to the same covariate depending on the scale of availability [20, 21]. This also applies to human infrastructure, e.g. wolves (Canis lupus) avoid gravel roads within their home range but select gravel roads on a local scale [22]. Spatial scale can also be of importance for movement and habitat selection during different life stages, because dispersers navigating a novel landscape will likely only know what is in its immediate surroundings and have less knowledge at a landscape scale.

Here we use the brown bear (Ursus arctos) as a model species within a human-modified landscape in Sweden to study the impact of human infrastructure, i.e. roads and buildings, movement and habitat selection at the landscape and local scale during two life stages. Like other large carnivores, brown bears have large home ranges [23] and can travel long distances [24]. There is ample scientific evidence that humans influence brown bear behaviour [25,26,27], and bears are able to perceive and respond to local context-specific risks [28], but it is unknown how dispersers navigate human-modified environments compared with residents. In this study, we focus on male brown bears in two life stages, dispersers, and residents. We estimate habitat selection and movement at the landscape and local scale and evaluate whether the effect of human infrastructure differs between the two life stages.

We hypothesize (H1) that human infrastructure influence movement of dispersing and resident male brown bears. In support of (H1), we predict (P1) that the most parsimonious model explaining movement and habitat selection for both dispersers and residents will include buildings or roads at one or both spatial scales. We hypothesize (H2) that dispersers will be more naïve or risk-tolerant compared with residents. In support of H2, we predict (P2) that dispersers will show less avoidance of or be closer to buildings and roads compared with residents. We hypothesize (H3) that bears of both life stages will be more sensitive to human infrastructure at the local scale than the landscape scale. In support of H3, we predict (P3) that bears in each life stage will exhibit stronger avoidance of buildings and roads at the local scale compared to the landscape scale.

Methods

Study area and study species

The study area is located in southcentral Sweden (approximately 61° N, 15° E), primarily within Gävleborg and Dalarna counties, spanning ~ 50,000 km2 (Fig. 1A). The landscape consists of boreal forest, bogs, lakes, and sparse agricultural land. The intensively managed forest is dominated by Norway spruce (Picea abies) and Scots pine (Pinus sylvestris) [29]. Rolling hills comprise the general topography, with steeper and more rugged terrain in the western portion of the study area (elevation range 0–997 m a.s.l.). Human settlement in the area is sparse, with an average of 8.64 inhabitants/km2 and people tend to live in small villages [30]. There are several larger towns and cities, but urban areas and sub-urban settings contribute to small fractions of our study area and few bears are exposed to this magnitude of human infrastructure. There is an extensive road network, dominated by gravel roads used for forestry. The road density is low in a European context [31] and the traffic volumes are low in a Swedish perspective. Almost all roads are unfenced, only the largest public roads are fenced.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Diagram shows A study area within Sweden. B Differences between GPS locations of resident (gold) and dispersing (blue) male brown bears. C Availability space and sampling design for landscape scale for resource selection function (RSF) where grey dots represent available location and black points represent used. D Availability and sampling design for local scale for integrated step selection analysis (iSSA), where grey arrows represent available steps and black arrows represent used steps

Current brown bear density in the study area ranges from ca 20 to 60 bears per 1000 km2 [32]. Scandinavian brown bears are subject to high hunting pressure and approximately 70% of the total mortality is due to legal hunting [32]. They generally avoid humans and their settlements [33]. By way of human infrastructure, brown bears are exposed to mostly roads and buildings in our study area. Male brown bears have large home ranges, on average 800 km2 [34], encompassing all types of human infrastructure, which they generally avoid [35]. We focused on males because dispersal is primarily male-biased (94%) [36].

Telemetry data

Brown bears were captured, collared, and monitored from 2007 to 2017 as part of a long-term research project [37]. See Arnemo and Evans [38] for a more detailed description of capture and handling procedures. Capture and handling of bears was conducted by permit under Swedish authorities and ethical committees. Bears were fitted with GPS collars (GPS Plus, Vectronic Aerospace GmbH) with different programming schedules, but all were scheduled to acquire at least one GPS location each hour. All GPS locations were resampled to one location every hour (± 3 min tolerance). We retained only GPS locations with a dilution of precision (DOP) of less than 10 to reduce location error [39]. All GPS locations overlaying water bodies were removed prior to analyses. As our focus was on movement and habitat selection in relation to human infrastructure, we removed locations associated with resting sites (day and night beds) [28]. We defined a bed site as a cluster of GPS locations with a maximum distance of 50 m between any two GPS locations in the cluster, a maximum distance of 30 m between two consecutive GPS locations and at least 5 consecutive locations, i.e., the bear had to spend at least 4 h in the same location to be defined as a bed site.

Defining dispersing and resident bear-years

We focused only on natal dispersal, i.e. the permanent movement from birth site to first breeding, and will hereafter refer to it as dispersal. We visually examined the GPS tracks of every bear-year, i.e. the unique combination of bear ID and year, to identify bear-years with diagnostic linear tracks typical of dispersal events (Fig. 1B). This approach might underestimate dispersal in males that gradually move away from their maternal range over multi-year periods, however, this behavior is difficult to disentangle from home range drift or infidelity [40]. In addition, previous studies have detected high rates (> 92%) of male dispersal [36, 41]. We performed ‘path segmentation’ [42] on movement tracks of bear-years identified with a dispersal event and used hidden Markov models (HMM), a form of state-space modeling [43], to define the transient period of dispersal. Hence, this method identified the onset and end of the dispersal event. For each track, we fit seven HMMs that varied in the number of states and the initial parameters (see Additional file 1: supplement S1 for more details on model fitting and structures). We selected the most parsimonious (hereafter ‘best’) model using Akaike’s Information Criterion [44] and used the Viterbi algorithm [45] to classify behaviors from the best model. Based on the classified behaviors, we defined the onset and end for the dispersal period for the bear-years identified as dispersing (Additional file 1: supplement S1). Only one dispersal period was defined for each of the bear-years identified as dispersing, and only data from this period for each bear-year was used in the further analysis for dispersers. We used the R package ‘moveHMM’ [46] for fitting HMMs, model selection, and behavioral classification. Dispersal phases lasted from 21 to 65 days (mean: 43 ± 15) for the 15 males (15 bear-years) defined as dispersing. During their dispersal events, these bears ranged in age from 2 to 4 years old (mean 2.7 years, n = 15).

We defined resident bear-years as all years that a GPS-collared bear had been solitary for at least three years (Fig. 1B), and no dispersal event had been detected. Males separate from their mother at 1 or 2 years of age [47] and Støen, Zedrosser [41] showed that 92.3% of male bears (n = 67) had dispersed by four years of age in the study area, with no observed dispersal events at 4 years of age. Hence, based on our definition of a resident bear-year, we avoided the inclusion of solitary pre-dispersing males, i.e. young males that had left their mother but not dispersed yet, as resident bear-years. We defined an active period between 25 April and 20 August which follows den emergence and precedes the hunting season for brown bears in Sweden. Brown bears in Sweden are known to change their movement pattern after the onset of hunting [48]. For residents, we excluded all data outside this period, and only individuals with GPS locations covering at least 70% of the days during the active period each year for further analysis. For dispersers there were two cases which extended outside this period: one started dispersing 21 April and another whose dispersal ended 22 August. To maximize disperser sample size, we included the six days of data outside the active period for these two individuals. We identified 20 males spanning 46 bear-years that met our resident criteria. The resident bear-years ranged in age from 4 to 21 years old (mean = 9.7 years, n = 45, one with unknown age but classified as adult based on head circumference).

Covariates

We included the following ‘core’ covariates reported to influence brown bear habitat selection and movement in our analysis: terrain ruggedness index (TRI) [35], clearcuts [49], bogs [50], and distance to water [51, 52]. We calculated TRI from a digital elevation model (25-m resolution) with the R package spatialEco [53] using a 3 × 3 cell moving window. We obtained data on landcover, roads, and buildings from the Swedish Mapping, Cadastral and Land Registration Authority [54]. In Sweden, forestry practitioners must report timber harvesting activities [55], and we used the data for defining clearcuts as logged areas from first cutting up to 10 years [56]. We created a clearcut raster (presence = 1, absence = 0) for each year of the study.

We divided our road data into forestry and public roads. Forestry roads represent the majority of the road network in the study area (mean = 1.27 ± 1.07 km/km2), which are mainly small gravel roads built for forestry and usually open for the public. Public roads are larger and mostly paved and associated with higher traffic volume (mean = 0.18 km/km2, SD = 0.45). We included all buildings, the majority of which were houses or cabins (mean density = 10.7 ± 39.4 buildings/km2). We rasterized all covariates to a resolution of 25 m. We calculated the Euclidean distance from all cells in the raster to the nearest forestry road, public road, and building, for each feature separately. All distance to covariates were log transformed prior to analysis to attenuate covariate effects at longer distances.

Habitat selection and movement analyses

We used exponential resource selection functions (RSF) to estimate habitat selection at the landscape scale (availability defined by the study area extent) [57], and integrated step selection analysis (iSSA) to estimate habitat selection and movement parameters at the local scale (availability defined by hourly relocations) [58]. We used the bear locations excluding bed sites for RSF and iSSA. RSF and iSSA models were fitted at the bear-year level, i.e. one model for each bear-year. The estimates from each model on the bear-year level were later averaged to obtain one population estimate for dispersing individuals and one for resident individuals.

For the RSF, the availability space was defined as the 100% minimum convex polygon of all observed GPS locations for all bear-years buffered by the radius of a circular mean male home range size (r =  ~ 18 km). For each bear-year, we randomly sampled available GPS locations from the availability space with a ratio of 20:1 available-to-use (Fig. 1C). The RSF was obtained by fitting a generalized linear model with the glm function in R. The GPS locations were used as the response variable and coded ‘1’ for used and ‘0’ for available.

An iSSA is a form of step selection functions that simultaneously estimate habitat selection and movement parameters [58]. We created “steps” by combining two consecutive GPS locations that were not part of a bed site. An iSSA requires at least two valid consecutive steps, as turning angles need to be calculated and included in model structure. Every step had a duration of 1 h. An iSSA uses ‘local scale availability’, i.e. locations to which an animal could possibly have moved to in a given step. Based on the used steps, we calculated step lengths and fitted a gamma probability density function based on maximum likelihood estimation for both life stages combined. We drew 20 step lengths from the fitted gamma distribution for each used step (20:1 available-to-use ratio) and combined them with turning angles drawn from a uniform distribution to generate ‘available’ steps (Fig. 1D). We assigned a unique stepID to each used step and its 20 associated available steps. The available steps represent what was locally available to bears at the starting point of every step. The iSSA was modelled using conditional logistic regression with used (coded as 1) and available steps (coded as 0) as the response variable, and the stepID as the stratum (for matching the used and available steps). Covariates for used and available steps were extracted at the start- and endpoints of the step. The covariates extracted at endpoints are used to infer habitat selection, while the covariates extracted at starting points together with an interaction with step length (or the logarithm of step length) are used to infer movement speed. See Avgar, Potts [58] for a detailed description of iSSA.

Candidate models and model selection

For each RSF and iSSA, we developed candidate models for all dispersers and residents. Each candidate model set contained a model including a set of ‘core covariates’. Core covariates account for habitat features which previously have been shown to be important for bear habitat selection and movement. We chose the combination of core covariates as they should cover the most prevalent habitat classes in our study area. The core model was extended with additional covariates representing specific human infrastructure attributes to form competing candidate models. We also included a ‘full’ model containing all covariates into the analysis (Table 1). RSF and iSSA candidate model formulae were identical, except iSSA models included movement-related covariates (Table 1). For iSSA, we included step lengths (SL) and the natural logarithm of step lengths (lnSL) in all models to capture movement differences between life stages. All covariates were standardized with the formula (Xmean of X)/standard deviation of X and checked for collinearity. The highest correlation was 0.30, reported for the variables distance to public roads and distance to buildings.

Table 1 Candidate models used to test the relative importance of forestry roads, public roads, and buildings and habitat selection and movement in male brown bears

We performed model selection for each bear-year for both the RSF and iSSA models. We calculated AIC for all models for each bear-year and calculated the delta AIC from the best model for all candidate models within bear-years. We summed the delta AIC for all the candidate models and considered the model with the lowest mean AIC for each life stage as the best model for that life stage. Note that this may cause the best model to have a mean delta AIC > 0, because that model structure might not be ‘best’ across all bear-years for a given life stage and scale.

Population level effects

To infer habitat selection and movement responses at the population level, i.e. for dispersers and residents, we averaged the bear-year models using inverse variance-weighted linear modelling [59], following the approach by Dickie, McNay [60]. We fitted inverse-variance linear regression models separately for residents and dispersers and for each RSF and iSSA model set. We used either RSF or iSSA coefficients as response variables and included the mean availability of each variable as an explanatory variable for a given bear-year to control for a possible functional response [61]. We used the inverse of the estimated variance for the coefficients as weights. The availability used in the inverse variance-weighted linear regression models was centered (x—mean of x), to aid in interpretation. The population-level coefficients can be interpreted as the mean coefficient at the mean availability for the males in the population (each for dispersers and residents and for the RSF and iSSA). We interpreted coefficients with 95% CIs overlapping with zero as ‘indifferent’ and non-overlapping 95% CIs as significant avoidance or selection, or as an effect of the covariate on movement rate [60]. We also recorded the direction of the coefficients for all individual bear-year models and reported the proportion of bear-year models that followed the same direction as the mean of all bear-years in a given life stage. This measure reflects the consistency of individual responses to covariates for each life stage between the local and the landscape scale for the population.

In iSSA, the estimated coefficients for SL, lnSL, and their interactions function as modifiers for the initial estimates of the scale and shape parameters, respectively, in the fitted gamma distribution (used for sampling the available step lengths) on step lengths [58]. For each bear-year, we adjusted the shape and scale parameters of the fitted gamma distribution. We calculated movement rates at the bear-year level by multiplying the adjusted shape and adjusted scale parameters from the gamma distribution. To illustrate changes in movement rates, we calculated movement rates at several levels for each of the focal covariates and kept all other interacting covariates (with lnSL) constant at their mean observed step value. Movement rates at the population level were obtained by calculating the mean of individual bear-year movement rates across the different levels of a given focal variable and for each life stages separately (iSSA only).

For both RSF and iSSA results, we calculated the relative selection strength (RSS) following Avgar, Lele [62] for all covariates in each analysis. The RSS was calculated based on the population-averaged estimates from the inverse-variance weighted linear models (one for each life stage and availability scale). For step selection functions (iSSA), the ln RSS is a relative measure of how likely the individual is to select a step that ends at location x1 in relation to a step that ends at location x2 (the reference location). For “distance to feature” covariates, we calculated the RSS moving one mean step length closer to the feature compared with staying at the same location, and for the other covariates we calculated the RSS of selecting a given feature over the mean of the covariate.

All distance calculations were performed in GRASS 7.2 [63]. We used the ‘amt’ package [64] for iSSA and R 3.6.0 all other statistical analyses [65].

Results

The bed removal procedure removed 28% of the GPS locations, leaving 70,008 GPS locations for statistical analysis (8012 GPS locations for dispersers and 61,996 GPS locations for residents). The bed removal procedure biased removal of more observations during day (Additional file 2: Fig. S1). Mean step lengths were 729 m for dispersers and 586 m for residents. Used locations rarely (< 7%) occurred in bogs and clearcuts (Table 2), and the mean distance to water was 705 m for dispersers and 712 m for residents. There were large differences between dispersers and residents in the mean values of their used locations for human covariates (Table 2). Used locations of dispersers and residents occurred on average 788 and 1186 m from buildings, 3014 and 5213 m from public roads, and 250 and 294 m from forestry roads, respectively.

Table 2 Values for covariates at used locations for brown bears defined as residents and dispersers in Sweden

The full models had the lowest mean delta AIC at both spatial scales for dispersers (landscape scale ΔAIC = 0.82, local scale ΔAIC = 2.06) as well as residents (landscape scale ΔAIC = 0.32, local scale ΔAIC = 0.96). The full model scored the lowest AIC for 40% and 20% of the bear-years for the dispersers at the landscape scale and local scale, respectively (Table 3). The full model scored the lowest AIC for 78% and 63% of the bear-years for the residents at the landscape scale and local scale, respectively (Table 3). The four best RSF (landscape scale) models for both life stages contained the covariate public roads, whereas the four best iSSA models (local scale) for both life stages contained the covariate forestry roads.

Table 3 Model selection for dispersing and resident male brown bears of model sets fit using resource selection functions (RSFs) and integrated step selection analysis (iSSA)

Core covariates

At the population level, dispersers and residents avoided bogs at both spatial scales, showing the same pattern for > 91% of the bear-years in each model (Table 4, Additional file 2: Fig. S2). Both dispersers and residents avoided clearcuts locally but were indifferent to them on the landscape scale (Table 4; Fig. 2). Dispersers and residents were indifferent to distance to water on the landscape scale, but residents selected for distances farther from water at the local scale (Table 4; Fig. 2). Dispersers were indifferent to TRI at either scale, however, residents selected for higher TRI at both scales (Table 4). In general, dispersers showed more individual variation and less consistency in their habitat selection towards the core covariates, i.e. a lower proportion of all bear-year estimates conformed to the same direction as the population mean effect, for dispersers compared with residents (Table 4; dispersers: mean of all proportions = 0.59, range = 0.333–0.933; residents: mean of all proportions = 0.79, range = 0.391–0.957). Only residents moved faster when their step started in a clearcut (Fig. 3D).

Table 4 Average habitat selection and movement rate coefficients for dispersers and residents at the landscape (RSF) and local scale (iSSA)
Fig. 2
figure 2

Relative selection strength (RSS) for dispersing and resident male brown bears from the inverse-variance weighted linear model. Column A is the RSS of staying in the habitat reference category (i.e. not bogs or clearcuts) compared with selecting for either bogs or clearcuts. Columns B, C, D and E show the RSS of moving closer towards the features (leftward) versus staying at the same distance across a range of starting distances (x-axis). Column F illustrates the RSS of selecting a given TRI value (x-axis) over the mean value (4.19). * indicates covariate was significant for respective ‘Resident’ and ‘Dispersal’ models (95% CIs did not overlap zero) for the curve that it is nearest. In cases where placement is not obvious, clarification is given in parenthesis

Fig. 3
figure 3

Mean movement rates (in meters/hour) for dispersing and resident male brown bears in relation to A distance to forestry roads, B distance to public roads with the same range as for forestry roads, C distance to buildings, D and whether or not the bear started in a clearcut. The focal variable has been varied while all other variables have been kept constant the mean of all used starting points and outside a clearcut for A, B and C. The asterisks * near ‘Resident’ and ‘Dispersal’ curves indicate that respective model estimates were significant (95% CIs did not overlap zero)

Human infrastructure

At the population level and across scales, dispersers were indifferent to human infrastructure, except for buildings which they avoided at the local scale. Residents avoided buildings and public roads, and selected forestry roads at both local and landscape scale. Residents did not alter their movement rate closer to buildings, while dispersers moved faster closer to buildings (Table 4; Fig. 3C). Both dispersers and residents increased their movement rate closer to forestry roads and closer to public roads (Table 4).

Dispersers and residents moved 551 m/h and 599 m/h, respectively, faster when their step started on a forestry road compared with the movement rate when starting 500 m away from a forestry road (Fig. 3A). Similarly, dispersers and residents moved 141 m/h and 169 m/h, respectively, faster when their step started on a public road compared with the movement rate at 500 m away from a public road (Fig. 3B). In other words, the change in the movement speed for dispersers and residents was 3.26 and 3.54 times higher, respectively, on forestry roads compared with public roads.

Discussion

We found evidence that human infrastructure is important for describing habitat selection and movement (P1 in support of H1) regardless of life stage (i.e. resident or disperser) at both the local and landscape scale, as models containing human infrastructure performed best. However, at the landscape scale, dispersers appeared indifferent towards most landscape features, including human infrastructure (Table 4 and Additional file 2: Fig. S2). Dispersers were more often indifferent towards human infrastructure at both scales (P2 in support of H2) and did not show the same avoidance patterns of human infrastructure as residents. Dispersers were less sensitive towards human infrastructure on the landscape scale than the local scale, however, this did not apply to residents (P3 partial support of H3). Furthermore, dispersers more often exhibited indifference to human infrastructure in both habitat selection and movement rates.

Human infrastructure appears important for both dispersers and resident at the local scale and the landscape scale (H1). At the local scale, dispersers and residents avoided buildings, and dispersers moved faster closer to buildings suggesting they are perceived as risky habitat. The lack of increased movement rate closer to buildings by residents might be due to residents generally being farther away from buildings. Both dispersers and residents moved faster when closer to public and forestry roads. Whether a road facilitates or impedes movement is likely dependent on the traffic volume [66]. This was presumably the case in our study area, as residents appeared to treat public roads as risky habitat (avoiding and moving faster), whereas they used the smaller forestry roads for travel (selecting and moving faster). The forestry roads are assumed to generally have low levels of human activity, and even lower at night when bears moved more (Additional file 2: Fig. S1). Mortality risk for bears along all roads during our study period was likely low, as traffic accidents do not account for a large proportion of mortality [< 2%; 67] and our study ended before the onset of the hunting season, after which roads have an additive effect on hunting success [68]. Movement facilitation in relation to linear features, such as roads, has also been observed for brown bears in other areas [60], while treating public roads as risky has been reported for other species [69, 70]. Dispersers were indifferent in terms of habitat selection for either road type and traveled faster near both, indicating that public and forestry roads may potentially serve as risky features [71] or as facilitators of movement [72] during dispersal. This indifference might be explained by the dispersers lack of information on where to find roads, or large variation in the population of dispersers where some avoid roads while others may select for them.

Although the best models at the landscape scale contained human infrastructure for both life stages (support of H1), dispersers were mainly indifferent to human infrastructure. They also used habitat closer to human infrastructure compared with residents. This is in support of H2, i.e. dispersers are either more naïve or risk-tolerant to human infrastructure. In contrast, residents were sensitive to human infrastructure and avoided both public roads and buildings at both availability scales. Hence, we suggest that naivety or risk-tolerance plays a prominent role in the behavior of dispersers when navigating novel landscapes. Dispersal is often considered risky due to movement through novel landscapes [73], and human-derived risks can have an additive effect beyond what is ‘normally expected’ by animals when weighing the decision whether to disperse and where to go [13]. In contrast, the assumption that residents are more familiar with their home ranges and dispersers must navigate novel terrain, is supported by the residents’ avoidance of potential human-derived risk at both local and landscape scales.

Alternatively, areas with lower human mortality risk might not be preferred by dispersers, due to increased intraspecific mortality risk or competition from larger, adult males [74, 75], which would effectively sandwich dispersing males between two sources of mortality risk [sensu] [76], i.e. humans and conspecifics. This is also consistent with our core covariate findings, i.e., residents selected for rugged terrain, while dispersers did not. Similarly, clearcuts and bogs are open habitats in which bears have less cover from human detection [77]. Such habitats were avoided by both dispersers and residents at the local scale, indicating a similar avoidance of potential human-derived risk in these open or semi-open habitat types. Dispersing males in other large carnivores, such as African lions (Panthera leo) [8], African wild dogs (Lycaon pictus) [78] and gray wolves [7], depict a similar pattern with weaker or no avoidance of human infrastructure. Lack of avoidance of human infrastructure may be driven by the attempt to avoid of larger males. However, it could also be explained by the dispersing individual’s inability to detect these features at relevant distances. Similarly, the lack of selection for landscape features preferred by older males, e.g. the lack of selection for rugged terrain by dispersers, may also be explained by the dispersing males not knowing where to find these features when moving through the landscape. This is supported by dispersers not selecting for any features on any of the scales. Alternatively, adult male habitat selection may emerge from learning, i.e. adjusting from being a dependent with its mother to that of an adult male.

Our selection of covariates might be more applicable toward residents, as it is based on the literature where more information is available for residents compared to dispersers. However, dispersers likely consider and respond to similar habitat features as residents, even though responses may differ. This is supported by our results, as the full model was best for both life stages and candidate models had identical sorting based on the mean ∆AIC and at both spatial scales (Table 4). We attribute the higher proportion of non-significant coefficients in the disperser models to higher individual variation among dispersers. The more risk-tolerant or naïve behavior in dispersers [7, 8] combined with a high level of individual variation may be important to maintain structural and functional connectivity between populations, as bolder or more risk-tolerant individuals [79] may disperse more effectively through human-modified landscapes. At the same time, this also implies that areas important for connectivity with a high human footprint may experience higher levels of human-bear conflict with implications for functional connectivity, unless carefully designed mitigation strategies are adopted [80].

We found partial support of H3, that the scale of availability influences how sensitive bears are towards human infrastructure, as only dispersers were more selective at the local scale compared with the landscape scale (P3). This is in contrast to what has been observed in pronghorn antelopes (Antilocapra americana) which showed stronger avoidance at the landscape scale than the local scale during migration [81], illustrating the existence of different strategies for which spatial scale animals avoid human infrastructure. Furthermore, dispersing brown bear males more often treated human infrastructure indifferently in habitat selection and movement rates at the local scale (Table 4). In our study area, bears are intensively hunted annually [upwards of 10% of the population; 82], putting them into contact with human-derived risk virtually everywhere in the study area, and bears modify their behavior to minimize human predation risk [17, 48, 83, 84]. Hence, resident bears likely select areas where human impact is low [35] and further reduce potential encounter rates with humans through behavioral changes on local spatial scales and temporally [28, 48]. As dispersers were more exposed to human infrastructure, they are potentially also more exposed to higher human predation risk. Indeed, hunting mortality of males compared to females appears to be higher at the onset of dispersal in this bear population [68, 85], which could partially be due to male-biased dispersal [41], increased risk-taking or tolerance during dispersal, and the inability of dispersing males to adequately recognize and adjust to novel human-derived mortality risk.

Conclusions

Our study highlights that life stage can influence how individuals respond to human landscape features across scales. The key to attaining reliable connectivity models is the recognition that animal dispersal decisions and movement patterns are life stage dependent. Our findings suggest that risk-tolerant or naïve dispersers might use movement pathways that more risk-averse or habitat-familiar residents would avoid. As a result, landscape resistance or connectivity maps derived from dispersal movement data may provide more numerous as well as more realistic pathways than those derived from resident movement data alone [86,87,88]. At the same time, the increased risk-tolerance or naivety in dispersers has the potential to exacerbate human-bear conflicts in important connectivity areas. Hence, identifying these areas early on from connectivity maps can help to mitigate human-bear conflict. The differences in scale-specific decisions between dispersers and residents provide the foundation for understanding functional connectivity of a population, in which animals disperse and establish a home range for subsequent reproduction [89]. Attaining individual-based dispersal data on large carnivores is costly but informative, particularly in human-dominated systems where coexistence has been touted as not only possible, but imperative on some level for large carnivore persistence [90,91,92].

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

References

  1. Tucker MA, et al. Moving in the anthropocene: global reductions in terrestrial mammalian movements. Science. 2018;359(6374):466–9.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Wilmers CC, et al. Scale dependent behavioral responses to human development by a large predator, the puma. PLOS ONE. 2013;8(4):11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Trombulak SC, Frissell CA. Review of ecological effects of roads on terrestrial and aquatic communities. Conserv Biol. 2000;14(1):18–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Zeller KA, McGarigal K, Whiteley AR. Estimating landscape resistance to movement: a review. Landsc Ecol. 2012;27(6):777–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Brennan A, et al. Functional connectivity of the world’s protected areas. Science. 2022;376(6597):1101–4.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Clobert J. Dispersal ecology and evolution. 1st ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2012.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  7. Barry T, et al. Does dispersal make the heart grow bolder? Avoidance of anthropogenic habitat elements across wolf life history. Anim Behav. 2020;166:219–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Elliot NB, et al. The devil is in the dispersers: predictions of landscape connectivity change with demography. J Appl Ecol. 2014;51(5):1169–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Abrahms B, et al. Does wildlife resource selection accurately inform corridor conservation? J Appl Ecol. 2017;54(2):412–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Gaynor KM, et al. Landscapes of fear: spatial patterns of risk perception and response. Trends Ecol Evol. 2019;34(4):355–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Gaynor KM, et al. The influence of human disturbance on wildlife nocturnality. Science. 2018;360(6394):1232–5.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Bonte D, et al. Costs of dispersal. Biol Rev. 2012;87(2):290–312.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Johnson CA, et al. Mortality risk increases with natal dispersal distance in American martens. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2009;276(1671):3361–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Thurfjell H, Ciuti S, Boyce MS. Learning from the mistakes of others: how female elk (Cervus elaphus) adjust behaviour with age to avoid hunters. PLOS ONE. 2017;12(6):e0178082.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Ciuti S, et al. Human selection of elk behavioural traits in a landscape of fear. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2012;279(1746):4407–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Thurfjell H, Ciuti S, Boyce MS. Applications of step-selection functions in ecology and conservation. Mov Ecol. 2014;2(1):4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Hertel AG, et al. Temporal effects of hunting on foraging behavior of an apex predator: Do bears forego foraging when risk is high? Oecologia. 2016;182(4):1019–29.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Fahrig L. Non-optimal animal movement in human-altered landscapes. Funct Ecol. 2007;21(6):1003–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Johnson DH. The comparison of usage and availability measurements for evaluating resource preference. Ecology (Washington, DC). 1980;61(1):65–71.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Ciarniello LM, et al. Grizzly bear habitat selection is scale dependent. Ecol Appl. 2007;17(5):1424–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. DeCesare NJ, et al. Transcending scale dependence in identifying habitat with resource selection functions. Ecol Appl. 2012;22(4):1068–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Zimmermann B, et al. Behavioral responses of wolves to roads: scale-dependent ambivalence. Behav Ecol. 2014;25(6):1353–64.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. McLoughlin PD, Ferguson SH, Messier F. Intraspecific variation in home range overlap with habitat quality: a comparison among brown bear populations. Evol Ecol. 2000;14(1):39–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Bartoń KA, et al. Bears without borders: long-distance movement in human-dominated landscapes. Global Ecology and Conservation. 2019;17:e00541.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Lodberg-Holm HK, et al. A human-induced landscape of fear influences foraging behavior of brown bears. Basic Appl Ecol. 2019;35:18–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Le Grand L, et al. Behavioral and physiological responses of scandinavian brown bears (Ursus arctos) to dog hunts and human encounters. Front Ecol Evol. 2019;7:9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Skuban M, Find’o S, Kajba M. Bears napping nearby: daybed selection by brown bears (Ursus arctos) in a human-dominated landscape. Can J Zool. 2018;96(1):1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Ordiz A, et al. Predators or prey? Spatio-temporal discrimination of human-derived risk by brown bears. Oecologia. 2011;166(1):59–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Martin J, et al. Coping with human disturbance: spatial and temporal tactics of the brown bear (Ursus arctos). Can J Zool. 2010;88(9):875–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Statistics Sweden. Human population. 2019; Available from: https://www.scb.se/vara-tjanster/oppna-data/oppna-geodata/statistik-pa-rutor/.

  31. Ibisch PL, et al. A global map of roadless areas and their conservation status. Science. 2016;354(6318):1423–7.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Bischof R, et al. Estimating and forecasting spatial population dynamics of apex predators using transnational genetic monitoring. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2020;117(48):30531–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Steyaert S, et al. Ecological implications from spatial patterns in human-caused brown bear mortality. Wildl Biol. 2016;22(4):144–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Dahle B, Swenson JE. Home ranges in adult Scandinavian brown bears (Ursus arctos): effect of mass, sex, reproductive category, population density and habitat type. J Zool. 2003;260(4):329–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Nellemann C, et al. Terrain use by an expanding brown bear population in relation to age, recreational resorts and human settlements. Biol Cons. 2007;138(1–2):157–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Zedrosser A, et al. Should I stay or should I go? Natal dispersal in the brown bear. Anim Behav. 2007;74:369–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Scandinavian Brown Bear Research Project. Available from: www.bearproject.info.

  38. Arnemo JM, Evans A. Biomedical protocols for free-ranging brown bears, wolves, wolverines and lynx. Norway: Hedmark University College Evenstad; 2017.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Frair JL, et al. Resolving issues of imprecise and habitat-biased locations in ecological analyses using GPS telemetry data. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2010;365(1550):2187–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Edwards MA, Nagy JA, Derocher AE. Low site fidelity and home range drift in a wide-ranging, large Arctic omnivore. Anim Behav. 2009;77(1):23–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Støen OG, et al. Inversely density-dependent natal dispersal in brown bears Ursus arctos. Oecologia. 2006;148:356–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Edelhoff H, Signer J, Balkenhol N. Path segmentation for beginners: an overview of current methods for detecting changes in animal movement patterns. Mov Ecol. 2016;4:21.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Langrock R, et al. Flexible and practical modeling of animal telemetry data: hidden Markov models and extensions. Ecology. 2012;93(11):2336–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Akaike H. A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Trans Autom Control. 1974;19:716–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Zucchini W, MacDonald IL, Langrock R. Hidden Markov models for time series: an introduction using R. 2nd ed. Boca Raton: Chapman and Hall/CRC Press; 2016.

    Google Scholar 

  46. Michelot T, et al. moveHMM: an R package for the statistical modelling of animal movement data using hidden Markov models. Methods Ecol Evol. 2016;7(11):1308–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Dahle B, Swenson JE. Factors influencing length of maternal care in brown bears (Ursus arctos) and its effect on offspring. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2003;54(4):352–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Ordiz A, et al. Do bears know they are being hunted? Biol Cons. 2012;152:21–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Nielsen SE, Boyce MS, Stenhouse GB. Grizzly bears and forestry I. Selection of clearcuts by grizzly bears in west-central Alberta, Canada. For Ecol Manag. 2004;199(1):51–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Leclerc M, et al. Quantifying consistent individual differences in habitat selection. Oecologia. 2016;180(3):697–705.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Steyaert S, et al. Resource selection by sympatric free-ranging dairy cattle and brown bears Ursus arctos. Wildl Biol. 2011;17(4):389–403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Steyaert S, et al. Male reproductive strategy explains spatiotemporal segregation in brown bears. J Anim Ecol. 2013;82(4):836–45.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Evans JS. spatialEco. R package version 1.3–6. 2021.

  54. Fastighetskartan. Available from: https://www.lantmateriet.se/

  55. Naturvårdsverket, Svenska Marktäckedata: Produktbeskrivning. Utgåva 1.2 2014. Available from https://gpt.vic-metria.nu/data/land/SMD_produktbeskrivning_20140627.pdf. Accessed 16 October 2018.

  56. Hertel AG, et al. Bears and berries: species-specific selective foraging on a patchily distributed food resource in a human-altered landscape. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2016;70(6):831–42.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Manly BFJ, et al. Resource selection by animals: statistical design and analysis for field studies. Dordrecht: Springer; 2002.

    Google Scholar 

  58. Avgar T, et al. Integrated step selection analysis: bridging the gap between resource selection and animal movement. Methods Ecol Evol. 2016;7(5):619–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Murtaugh PA. Simplicity and complexity in ecological data analysis. Ecology. 2007;88(1):56–62.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Dickie M, et al. Corridors or risk? Movement along, and use of, linear features varies predictably among large mammal predator and prey species. J Anim Ecol. 2020;89(2):623–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Mysterud A, Ims RA. Functional responses in habitat use: availability influences relative use in trade-off situations. Ecology. 1998;79(4):1435–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Avgar T, et al. Relative selection strength: quantifying effect size in habitat- and step-selection inference. Ecol Evol. 2017;7(14):5322–30.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  63. GRASS Development Team. Geographic Resources Analysis Support System (GRASS) software, version 7.2. Open Source Geospatial Foundation; 2017.

  64. Signer J, Fieberg J, Avgar T. Animal movement tools (amt): R package for managing tracking data and conducting habitat selection analyses. Ecol Evol. 2019;9(2):880–90.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  65. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria; 2021.

  66. Northrup JM, et al. Vehicle traffic shapes grizzly bear behaviour on a multiple-use landscape. J Appl Ecol. 2012;49(5):1159–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Bischof R, et al. The magnitude and selectivity of natural and multiple anthropogenic mortality causes in hunted brown bears. J Anim Ecol. 2009;78(3):656–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Bischof R, et al. Regulated hunting re-shapes the life history of brown bears. Nat Ecol Evol. 2018;2(1):116–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Prokopenko CM, Boyce MS, Avgar T. Characterizing wildlife behavioural responses to roads using integrated step selection analysis. J Appl Ecol. 2017;54(2):470–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Passoni G, et al. Roads constrain movement across behavioural processes in a partially migratory ungulate. Mov Ecol. 2021;9(1):1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Proctor MF, et al. Effects of roads and motorized human access on grizzly bear populations in British Columbia and Alberta, Canada. Ursus. 2019;30(E2):16–39.

    Google Scholar 

  72. Roever CL, Boyce MS, Stenhouse GB. Grizzly bear movements relative to roads: application of step selection functions. Ecography. 2010;33(6):1113–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Long ES, et al. Risky movements? Natal dispersal does not decrease survival of a large herbivore. Ecol Evol. 2021;11(6):2731–40.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  74. Elfstrom M, et al. Do Scandinavian brown bears approach settlements to obtain high-quality food? Biol Cons. 2014;178:128–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Swenson JE, Dahle B, Sandergren F. Intraspecific predation in Scandinavian brown bears older than cubs-of-the-year. Ursus. 2001;12:81–91.

    Google Scholar 

  76. Lone K, et al. Living and dying in a multi-predator landscape of fear: roe deer are squeezed by contrasting pattern of predation risk imposed by lynx and humans. Oikos. 2014;123(6):641–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Sahlén V, et al. Behavioural differences between single Scandinavian brown bears (Ursus arctos) and females with dependent young when experimentally approached by humans. PLOS ONE. 2015;10(4):e0121576.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  78. O’Neill HMK, Durant SM, Woodroffe R. What wild dogs want: habitat selection differs across life stages and orders of selection in a wide-ranging carnivore. BMC Zoology. 2020;5(1):1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Cote J, et al. Personality-dependent dispersal: characterization, ontogeny and consequences for spatially structured populations. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2010;365(1560):4065–76.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  80. Can ÖE, et al. Resolving human-bear conflict: a global survey of countries, experts, and key factors. Conserv Lett. 2014;7(6):501–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  81. Jakes AF, et al. Multi-scale habitat assessment of pronghorn migration routes. PLOS ONE. 2020;15(12):e0241042.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  82. Frank SC, et al. Indirect effects of bear hunting: a review from Scandinavia. Ursus. 2017;28(2):150–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. Leclerc M, Zedrosser A, Pelletier F. Harvesting as a potential selective pressure on behavioural traits. J Appl Ecol. 2017;54(6):1941–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  84. Leclerc M, et al. Harvesting as a potential selective pressure on behavioural traits. J Appl Ecol. 2017;54:1941–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  85. Frank SC, et al. Harvest is associated with the disruption of social and fine-scale genetic structure among matrilines of a solitary large carnivore. Evol Appl. 2021;14(4):1023–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Blazquez-Cabrera S, et al. Influence of separating home range and dispersal movements on characterizing corridors and effective distances. Landsc Ecol. 2016;31(10):2355–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  87. Killeen J, et al. Habitat selection during ungulate dispersal and exploratory movement at broad and fine scale with implications for conservation management. Mov Ecol. 2014;2:15.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  88. Vasudev D, et al. From dispersal constraints to landscape connectivity: lessons from species distribution modeling. Ecography. 2015;38(10):967–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  89. Zeller K, et al. Understanding the importance of dynamic landscape connectivity. Land. 2020;9(9):303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  90. Chapron G, et al. Recovery of large carnivores in Europe’s modern human-dominated landscapes. Science. 2014;346(6216):1517–9.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  91. Lamb CT, et al. The ecology of human-carnivore coexistence. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2020;117(30):17876–83.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  92. Oriol-Cotterill A, et al. Landscapes of coexistence for terrestrial carnivores: the ecological consequences of being downgraded from ultimate to penultimate predator by humans. Oikos. 2015;124(10):1263–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Johannes Signer for advice during the design phase of this study, as well as discussions with John Odden. We also thank the many field personnel who captured and radio-marked bears, in particular Sven Brunberg, David Ahlqvist, and Andrea Friebe, and various veterinarians including Jon Arnemo and Alina Evans, among others. We thank Jon Swenson for his past leadership in SBBRP during this study’s data collection.

Funding

The Scandinavian Brown Bear Research Project (SBBRP) has a long-term funding primarily from the Swedish Environmental Protection Agency and the Norwegian Environment Agency. This study was funded through the Research Council of Norway (Grants 251112 and 281092), 2015–2016 BiodivERsA COFUND call for research proposals, with the national funders ANR (ANR-16-EBI3-0003), NCN (2016/22/Z/NZ8/00121), DLR-PT (01LC1614A), UEFISCDI (BiodivERsA3-2015–147-BearConnect (96/2016), and RCN (269863).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

AZ, JEH, NHT, OGS and SF conceived the idea. JEH, NHT and SF organized the data and did the statistical analysis. NHT lead the writing of the manuscript with significant contributions from JEH and SF. JK contributed to project administration, capture and collaring of bears. AZ, JK and OGS secured funding. All authors contributed significantly through comments and edits on the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to N. H. Thorsen or A. Zedrosser.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Permits for collaring brown bears were given by the Swedish authorities and ethical committees (Uppsala Djurförsöksetiska Nämnd: C40/3, C212/9, C47/9, C210/10, C7/12, C268/12, C18/15. Statens Veterinärmediciniska Anstalt, Jordbruksverket, Naturvårdsverket: Dnr 35-846/03, Dnr 412-7093-08 NV, Dnr 412-7327-09 Nv, Dnr 31-11102/12, NV-01758-14).

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1: Supplement S1.

Description for hidden Markov model (HMM) to define the transient period of dispersal.

Additional file 2: Figure S1

. The number of GPS locations at given hours of the day prior to bed removal (A) and after bed removal (B). Figure S2. Coefficient plots from resource selection functions (A) and the integrated step selection analysis (B). Yellow color indicate dispersing males and blue color indicate resident males. Table S1. The mean availability coefficient for the weighted linear models calculating the population estimates. Significant coefficients are indicated in bold and indicate a functional response, i.e. that the effect of the covariate variates with the availability. “D2.” is an abbreviation for “distance to”.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Thorsen, N.H., Hansen, J.E., Støen, OG. et al. Movement and habitat selection of a large carnivore in response to human infrastructure differs by life stage. Mov Ecol 10, 52 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40462-022-00349-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40462-022-00349-y

Keywords