Skip to main content

Deep vs shallow: GPS tags reveal a dichotomy in movement patterns of loggerhead turtles foraging in a coastal bay

Abstract

Background

Individual variation in movement strategies of foraging loggerhead turtles have been documented on the scale of tens to hundreds of kilometers within single ocean basins. Use of different strategies among individuals may reflect variations in resources, predation pressure or competition. It is less common for individual turtles to use different foraging strategies on the scale of kilometers within a single coastal bay. We used GPS tags capable of back-filling fine-scale locations to document movement patterns of loggerhead turtles in a coastal bay in Northwest Florida, U.S.A.

Methods

Iridium-linked GPS tags were deployed on loggerhead turtles at a neritic foraging site in Northwest Florida. After filtering telemetry data, point locations were transformed to movement lines and then merged with the original point file to define travel paths and assess travel speed. Home ranges were determined using kernel density function. Diurnal behavioral shifts were examined by examining turtle movements compared to solar time.

Results

Of the 11 turtles tagged, three tracked turtles remained in deep (~ 6 m) water for almost the entire tracking period, while all other turtles undertook movements from deep water locations, located along edges and channels, to shallow (~ 1–2 m) shoals at regular intervals and primarily at night. Three individuals made short-term movements into the Gulf of Mexico when water temperatures dropped, and movement speeds in the Gulf were greater than those in the bay. Turtles exhibited a novel behavior we termed drifting.

Conclusions

This study highlighted the value provided to fine-scale movement studies for species such as sea turtles that surface infrequently by the ability of these GPS tags to store and re-upload data. Future use of these tags at other loggerhead foraging sites, and concurrent with diving and foraging data, would provide a powerful tool to better understand fine-scale movement patterns of sea turtles.

Background

Animals move for a variety of reasons including migration, foraging, reproduction, dispersal and exploration. In the marine environment, animals live in heterogenous environments that often contain patchy resources [1, 2] therefore they must integrate physiological, ecological, and behavioral information to make daily movement decisions [3]. These movements can include long-distance migrations [4] and fine scale shifts in behavior due to changes in prey [5] or the presence of predators [6]. Much of the focus of animal movement studies has been on long-distance movement patterns [7, 8] and their connection to anthropogenic or climactic drivers [9]. For marine species in particular, quantifying fine-scale linkages between organisms and their resources has been more difficult. However, advances in biologging techniques can provide the detailed data needed to better understand those linkages [2, 10, 11].

Location data provided via both satellite-based Argos tags and acoustic telemetry generate spatial information for marine vertebrates, but each method has limitations. Error associated with locations from Argos tags makes it difficult to link fine-scale movements to environmental variables that drive those movements [12, 13] while acoustic telemetry data are limited to areas where receivers are present [14]. Acquisition of Global Positioning System (GPS) locations provides accurate spatial data that allows for high resolution examinations of movement patterns relative to an animal’s environment [1, 11]. Development of rapid acquisition of GPS data that is linked to the Argos system (e.g., Fastloc) has provided new insights into the fine-scale movements of marine organisms, including sea turtles, and can provide detailed data that spans months or even years [15]. Argos tags can store location data on the tag, allowing data to be transmitted for relatively long time periods (typically 10 days) after they are collected. However, the limited bandwidth of Argos (256 bits per uplink) means that limited amounts of data can be transmitted. Further, Argos tags typically have an external whip antenna which can be a point of weakness, shearing off so that data transmissions cease [16, 17]. Iridium tags provide a solution to some of these problems by containing a far greater bandwidth, allowing more data to be transmitted, and are capable of two-way messaging. If communication with the satellites is unavailable, the data are stored and then re-transmitted (i.e., backfilled) automatically at a later time when communication is restored, which is particularly useful for species such as sea turtles (Jim et al. 2022, Jang et al. 2024) that spend much of the time submerged or during times of high wave energy when transmitters have limited success at satellite communication [16, 18]. This feature is also available from Global Systems Mobile Communication (GSM) Cellular two-way tags however that technology relies on base stations to log data and as such would only be appropriate for terrestrial or coastal species (Matos et al. 2015). The downside to iridium tags is that they require significantly longer surface time compared to Argos tags to relay information (8.5 s vs. 0.3–0.9 s), although with two-way communication, receipt of the message is confirmed, while Argos tags repeat messages many times to improve the possibility of receipt. Additionally, Iridium tags have no external antenna which may reduce the frequency of satellite acquisitions but may also provide a benefit as breakage of external antennae is often a source of tag failure [15, 16]. As such, for some species, this amount of time at the surface may be insufficient for relaying location and other data via the Iridium network. Nevertheless, previous studies have shown that loggerhead turtles are an excellent candidate species for Iridium tracking [19,20,21]. Use of GPS telemetry has highlighted the patchiness of marine habitats [1, 22] and the complexity of factors that drive movement patterns of marine vertebrates [23]. Inclusion of Iridium-linked GPS tags, particularly with loggerhead turtles, could improve those studies.

Of particular interest is the understanding of movement patterns within an animal’s home range because these areas typically encompass the resources that are most critical to species survival [24,25,26]. In addition, information on home ranges is necessary for understanding ecological communities, determining location and size of marine protected areas and assessing the threat potential of an invasive organism [27,28,29]. For marine species, such as sea turtles, size and location of home ranges is frequently defined [4, 30, 31] however these characterizations have generally been conducted on the mesoscale level [4, 32, 33]. Fine-scale features, and the associated movements undertaken by the animal in response to them, are less well-known but have started to be revealed through Fastloc-GPS Argos tracking [15]. These movements are more difficult to measure and the fine-scale features, such as prey assemblages, are often less predictable [34], and these knowledge gaps can limit effectiveness of conservation actions [35].

Loggerhead sea turtles (Caretta caretta) establish both juvenile and adult foraging home ranges, which are often disparate but occasionally overlap [36]. These foraging sites are spatially and temporally patchy on multiple scales and this variability affects movements [1]. Variability in movements of juvenile and adult loggerheads have been documented, primarily in response to mesoscale features such as habitat use (e.g. oceanic versus neritic) and seasonal temperatures [13, 37]. Less is known about what drives fine-scale movements within juvenile loggerhead home ranges, particularly in areas also shared by adults. Here we use GPS tags to examine fine-scale movements of loggerhead turtles at a neritic foraging area. By closely examining daily movement patterns, we predicted turtles would move within their home ranges in response to environmental and habitat variables including water depth and tidal cycles.

Methods

Study site

St. Joseph Bay is located in northwest Florida in the northern Gulf of Mexico and encompasses approximately 260 km2. The greatest depths in the bay (max depth 13.3 m) are located in the middle and northern portions of the bay and consist primarily of sand and mud sediments, and the shallowest depths (~ 0.5 m) occur at the southern end [38]. Some of the most pristine seagrass beds in the state of Florida, dominated by Thalassia testudinum, fringe the majority of the bay, with extensive shoals covering the southern end [38]. Along the edges of the bay, these shallow (0.5–1.5 m) shoals are relatively narrow (~ 100–500 m) and drop off immediately into deeper (~ 6 m) waters. In the southern end, the expansive shoals are transected by a series of relatively deep (6–7 m) sandy-bottomed channels [39].

Turtle captures and tagging

Loggerhead turtles (n = 11) were captured by hand from a 19-ft Boston Whaler in St. Joseph Bay, Florida between August 2019 and June 2021 (Fig. 1). After being observed from the vessel, personnel jumped from the boat, grabbed ahold of the rear carapace of the turtle and brought the individual to the surface before transferring the captured animal to the boat for tagging. All captured turtles were individually marked with a metal.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Map of St. Joseph Bay in northwest Florida showing the movements of 10 loggerhead sea turtles recorded hourly with GPS tags from September 2019 – September 2021

Inconel tag placed along the trailing edge of each front flipper and a passive integrated transponder (PIT) tag placed subcutaneously in the left shoulder. Curved carapace length (CCL) of turtles was measured using a cloth tape measure. Weight was determined by placing the turtle in a harness and hanging the harness from a hand-held Pesola spring scale. The entire work up and satellite tagging process took approximately 1.5 h and all tagged turtles were released at their original capture site.

To quantify fine-scale movements and activity patterns, the turtles were continuously tracked with Iridium-linked GPS tags (SeaTrkr 4370, Telonics Inc., Mesa AZ) that were affixed to the carapace using cool-setting epoxy (Superbond). Before deployment, tags were lightly sanded and covered with anti-fouling paint (Pettit Hydrocoat 1240 Blue, Modern Recreational Technologies, LLC, Hickory, NC, USA), as satellite tags deployed in the northern Gulf of Mexico fail primarily due to biofouling [16]. We streamlined tag attachment materials to minimize drag effects [40] on turtle’s swimming ability. These tags contained quick-fix pseudoranging (QFP) technology that were capable of obtaining a GPS position in an average of six seconds during our field testing on sea turtles, which is approximately six times faster than a conventional GPS location with very little reduction in accuracy. This technology provides a “middle ground” between full GPS and snapshot GPS receivers such as Fastloc which can provide a much faster location (a few 10’s of milliseconds for Fastloc-GPS tags) at the cost of greater positional error (Our QFP tags provided 50% location accuracy of 6 m and 90% accuracy of 15 m with 6 satellites compared to 18 and 70 m respectively for Fastloc; [41, 42], making it possible to get extremely high positional accuracy on species that surface long enough for this technology to work, such as loggerhead sea turtles. The tags were programmed to obtain exclusively QFP to extend battery life, with a frequency of one location per hour. Position data and hourly water temperature data were periodically uploaded to the Iridium (Iridium Communications Inc., McLean, Virginia) satellite network for archiving and accessed for analysis using Telonics Data Converter (v. 2.80, Telonics Inc., Mesa AZ).

Data analysis

After downloading telemetry data with Telonics Data Converter, all further processing and analyses were produced with the statistical program R [v. 4.0.5; 43]. Spatial calculations were performed with package sf [44], data distributions were visualized with stat_slab in the package ggdist [45], and data visualization was performed with package ggplot2 [46]. Locations were projected from the GPS native World Geodetic System 1984 (WGS84) to EPSG: 26,916, North American Datum 1983 (NAD83), Universal Transverse Mercator zone 16 N (NAD16) to allow for measuring distances and areas in meters. Other spatial layers were also transformed where necessary to match the same projection. The NOAA Medium Resolution 1:70,000 scale Digital Vector Shoreline [47] was used for maps. Bathymetry data were obtained from Digital Elevation Model (DEM) data from the NOAA Continuously Updated Digital Elevation Model (CUDEM) − 1/9 Arc-Second tiles [48]. Turtle locations were overlaid on the DEM and the Mean Sea Level (MSL) depth was assigned to each point. Hourly water level data from the National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration (NOAA) station 8,729,108 in Panama City were then added to the elevation from the DEM to correct the water depth for each turtle telemetry location for current tide stage.

Telemetry locations with a horizontal dilution of precision > 25 were discarded. Speed and positional filters were then applied to further eliminate spurious locations such that any points where the travel speed was > 4 km per hour (km h− 1) were removed. Similarly, occasionally the tags would attempt an updated location if a poor-quality or unresolved QFP location was received. These duplicate locations, defined as points received within 10 min of each other, were identified, and the first location of each pair was discarded; the second location invariably being a higher-quality, updated location. The number of successful GPS locations per day was compared to the expected 24 attempts to measure daily success rate. Similarly, the number of successful locations each hour of the day compared to the number of days the transmitter was in operation allowed us to measure average success rate for daily time periods (e.g. day vs. night).

The point locations were converted to movement lines by joining successive points with line segments. The length of each line segment was calculated in meters by using the difference in UTM positions of the endpoints, the time to travel that distance was calculated as the difference between the GPS times of the endpoints, and travel speed was calculated by dividing distance by travel time. The polyline was then merged with the original point file so that the attributes of each travel line were assigned to the second of the two points used to draw the line. This second-point assignment associated the turtle’s destination with the travel paths.

Home range locations were determined by applying a kernel density function with standard deviation of 150 m to travel lines, filtered to a maximum speed of 0.08 km h− 1 and a maximum travel distance of 320 m, eliminating travel times > 4 h between successive locations. These were calculated with density.psp in the package spatstat [49]. Differences in travel rate were calculated using the function lmer and drifting rates were compared with a binomial model using function glmer (package lme4, [50]).

Preliminary observation of the turtle movements indicated behavioral shifts in several individuals that shifted near sunrise and sunset, so each 24 h period was split into day/night periods at the time of sunrise and sunset for each day as calculated by the function sunrise in the package maptools [51] for further analysis. We also explored changes in behavior during crepuscular times (here defined as two hours before to two hours after sunrise and sunset).

Results

Of the eleven tracked loggerheads, nine were juveniles (< 80 cm CCL; [39, 52]) and two (T4 91.0 and T9 92.2 cm CCL) were most likely adults [53, 111]. All eleven turtles were tracked at various times during the study period which extended from September 2019 through September 2021 (Fig. 2). All turtles returned useable numbers of locations (536–4942 per individual), except.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Timeline of locations received from 10 loggerhead sea turtles recorded hourly with GPS tags from September 2019 – September 2021

one individual that was removed from the data set because only one month of data was received, with fewer than 100 locations. The final number of turtle GPS locations after filtering was 13,303, of which 12,203 were within St. Joseph Bay (Table 1). Most turtles stayed in the bay, but three individuals left the bay during winter and returned after 1–2 months in the Gulf. Another turtle briefly exited the mouth of the bay on multiple occasions but did not travel into open water (Fig. 1).

Table 1 Summary table

Three of the tracked turtles remained in deep (> 5 m) water for almost all the tracking period, while all of the others made directed moves from deep water to shallow shoals (< 1.5 m) at regular intervals (Fig. 3). These turtles used shallow water habitats primarily at night. The deep-water locations used by turtles that made regular moves to shallow shoals were consistently at edges of shoals and in channels (Fig. 4).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Individual density graph of water depths, corrected for tide stage of the locations from 10 loggerhead sea turtles recorded hourly with GPS tags from September 2019 – September 2021. Data from locations within St. Joseph Bay only

Fig. 4
figure 4

Kernel density plot of local, short-range movements from 10 loggerhead sea turtles recorded hourly with GPS tags from September 2019 – September 2021. Concentrations of habitat use in the west and southwest of St. Joseph Bay, Florida can be seen along shoal edges and within deeper channels, as well as along the tops pf shoals near the shoreline

Movement rate for all of the tagged turtles was slightly faster during daylight hours compared to nighttime (Supplemental Fig.1). However, their home ranges were twice as large on average at night, indicating that they were selecting a wider variety of habitat patches during the hours of darkness. When crepuscular movements were considered, their movement speeds, habitat depths and locations were found to be intermediate, or mixtures of day and night measurements. Of the turtles that used shallow shoals, they moved closer to shore at night compared to the daytime, especially at high tide when access to the shoals was increased (Fig. 5; Supplemental Fig. 2). Shoal foraging depth averaged about 1 m at night, but the small amount of foraging on shoals that took place during the day was at about 2 m depth.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Combined density graph of water depths, corrected for tide stage of the locations from 10 loggerhead sea turtles recorded hourly with GPS tags from September 2019 – September 2021. Turtle movements were grouped by location and day/night, showing more use of shallow shoals at night, and at high tide stage

Home ranges for the loggerheads in St. Joseph Bay ranged from 0.52 to 4.02 km2 for the 50th percentile, and 2.64–20.69 km2 for the 90th percentile. The home range size was not related to the number of locations recorded, as the smallest home ranges were seen in turtles that recorded 262–2527 GPS locations, and the largest home ranges were seen in turtles that recorded 534–2235 GPS locations (Table 1). The home ranges showed very little overlap; instead, each turtle was located in distinct areas of the bay (Fig. 6) with 79% of the 50th percentile home ranges and 68% of the 90th percentile home ranges not overlapping with any other turtle (Table 2).

Fig. 6
figure 6

Kernel density plot of local, short-range movements from 10 loggerhead sea turtles recorded hourly with GPS tags from September 2019 – September 2021 within St. Joseph Bay, Florida. (a) Individual plots illustratingthe wide variety of individual home range locations, and (b) summed plots showing high use of the western shoreline

Table 2 Overlap of home ranges. Total area is the sum of all cells that are part of the home ranges of one or more turtles. Most home ranges did not overlap with those of other turtles

GPS location relay success rate was ~ 20–80% less during the day compared to night, especially in the morning hours from 8 to 12. This timing is similar to deep water to shallow shoals moves among those turtles that made those moves. The amount of data recovered after the initial transmission attempt with the Iridium re-transmission ability ranged from as little as ~ 5% to over 80%, recovering data from up to three months in the past. There was no apparent pattern in re-transmission related to turtle size or movement pattern: we suspect it was related to how high each transmitter antenna rode above the water surface, which depended on each turtle’s swimming attitude.

The median travel speed of the turtles within St. Joseph Bay was 0.046 km h− 1 (95% range 0.004–0.714). In the open Gulf, their speed was considerably faster, with a median of 0.238 km h− 1 (95% range 0.004–1.530; t = 17.1, p < 0.001; Supplemental Fig. 1). Within the bay, turtles traveled faster in water depths < 6 m (median 0.105 km h− 1; 95% range 0.006–0.740) than they did in deeper water (median 0.036 km h− 1; 95% range 0.004–0.684; t = 30.4, p < 0.001; Supplemental Fig. 1). They also travelled faster during the day (median 0.072 km h− 1; 95% range 0.005–1.026) vs. at night (median 0.038 km h− 1; 95% range 0.004–0.794; t = 21.9, p < 0.001). Warmer water temperatures were associated with higher travel speeds as well, with a 0.037 km h− 1 (SE = 0.003) increase in speed for each degree C (t = 13.6, p < 0.001; Supplemental Fig. 1).

While examining the movement data we encountered a recurring long-term slow movement we are calling “drifting” (Fig. 7). It was characterized by at least 12 h (up to 5 days in duration) of continuous slow movements (0.06 kmh-1) that upon visual review had small turning angles (< 20°), and high GPS reception rates (at least 50% successful). All of the turtles exhibited drifting behavior, but there was a wide range of prevalence among the turtles, from 1.4% of their time in only two identified drifting episodes (T-11) to 38.5% of their time in 46 episodes (T-03; Table 1). There was no apparent correspondence between habitat use behaviors (deep only vs. deep and shallow) and drifting behavior, but colder water temperatures were associated with more drifting (e.g. 36.2% below 16 °C vs. 4.7% above 28 °C; glmer z = 42.5, p < 0.001).

Fig. 7
figure 7

Travel maps and time series plots of loggerhead sea turtles recorded hourly with GPS tags from September 2019 – September 2021 highlighting a novel behavior we termed “drifting”. Panels a, c, and e show spatial movements of three turtles, and panels b, d and f show the corrected water depth of each turtle over time. The color of the points and lines indicate the speed of the turtle, and pink outlines highlight when the turtle was drifting. (a,b) Turtle T.03 showing daily movements to shallow shoals during high tide at night, alternating with periods of offshore deep-water drifting; (c,d) turtle T.01 spent its time in deep water, alternating between periods of activity and drifting, (e,f) turtle T.08 used shallow shoals mostly at night at high tide, similar to turtle T.03 but rarely showed drifting behavior

Discussion

Globally, loggerheads exhibit a dichotomy in basin-wide movement patterns with some individuals foraging in oceanic waters and others in neritic habitats [54,55,56,57]. Similar variation was also evident on the fine scale at a foraging area in St. Joseph Bay where juvenile and adult loggerheads exhibited three general movement patterns: (1) nocturnal movements onto shallow seagrass shoals during high tide, (2) consistent use of deeper waters, sometimes immediately adjacent to those shoals, and (3) use of shallow seagrass flats in the southern end of the bay during high tide. Some of these movement patterns were similar to those reported by Dujon et al. [1] from the Mediterranean Sea, and in both their study and ours, movement patterns varied by time of day, water depth and temperature. However, the movements we documented occurred on a relatively small spatial scale (i.e., within one coastal bay vs. within a sea or ocean basin). Additionally, our tagged loggerheads displayed some novel movements such as periodically drifting in deeper waters for multiple days, which they did more of in colder water temperatures (Fig. 7). Tagged loggerheads in our study established relatively small home ranges [1, 4, 58], similar in size to, but slightly smaller than, those reported by Lamont and Iverson [13], that overlapped only minimally among individuals. As has been documented with other sea turtle studies [19, 21, 59], the GPS tags used here provided us with relatively long-term and fine-scale movement data for adult and juvenile loggerheads.

It is likely that fixed foraging strategies have developed among individual loggerheads, as has been reported elsewhere [1, 56], that have resulted in different movement patterns. Individual variability in movement patterns exist across taxa and habitats [60, 61] and can reflect long-term behaviors (i.e., individual personality or life-history strategies; [56]) or short-term behavioral plasticity in response to changing environmental variables [60, 62, 63]. This variability can contribute to niche specialization [64]. As a species, loggerheads are considered generalist carnivores with a relatively large foraging niche [65] however individual specialization in diet has been documented [54, 66]. In St. Joseph Bay, individuals that used shallow shoals and deeper waters were most likely targeting benthic invertebrates (e.g., gastropods, crustaceans) whereas those that remained in deep water were either undertaking dives to the benthos to forage [62, 67, 68] or foraging on floating invertebrates such as tunicates and jellyfish [68, 69]. Individuals that remained in deep water were often located immediately adjacent to shoals but never actually moved onto those shallow-water habitats which suggests this is an individual strategy [56, 70] or there is a force such as intraspecific competition [1, 71] keeping those individuals off the shoals. In the eastern Gulf of Mexico, Silver-Gorges et al. [72] suggested adult loggerheads foraged and resided in shallow habitats, and juveniles moved between deeper locations to forage and shallower locations to rest. In their study, some juveniles however, also foraged and resided in shallow areas, adjusting behaviors to avoid intraspecific competition [72]. Similar movement patterns among adult and juvenile loggerheads were not documented at our site, however intraspecific competition could still occur [71]. In fact, home ranges of our tagged loggerheads only minimally overlapped which may suggest intraspecific competition (Table 1). Additionally, green turtle and Kemp’s ridley home ranges overlap with loggerheads in St. Joseph Bay [13] and interspecific competition may also impact loggerhead movement patterns [71].

For loggerheads that undertook movements between shallow shoals and deeper waters, predation risk most likely drove the diel pattern where turtles used deep waters during the day and shallow waters at night. Similar movement patterns have been observed in fish [73] and elasmobranchs [74]. Tiger sharks are known predators of sea turtles [6], are more active in the Gulf of Mexico during the day [75] and are present in St. Joseph Bay. In fact, we have observed tiger sharks foraging on sea turtles in shallow seagrass habitat in the bay. Although use of different diurnal home ranges has been reported in various sea turtle species, typically individuals used deeper refuges at night and shallower foraging habitats during the day [70, 76] which is in opposition to what we documented for our tagged loggerheads. Loggerheads in the Mediterranean exhibited a dichotomy in diurnal habitat use, with some individuals using deep waters at night and others using shallow night-time refuges [1]. Dujon et al. [1] suggested these differences reflected the availability of refuges (e.g., bathymetry, availability of structures, etc.) to avoid predators at each site. The lack of refuges (i.e., reefs and ledges; but see [71]) in St. Joseph Bay may drive the diurnal movement patterns documented here, as opposed to the pattern suggested by Silver-Gorges et al. [72] in the eastern Gulf of Mexico (e.g., adults resided in shallow waters) where reefs are more prevalent [77]. Our tracked loggerheads, regardless of foraging strategy, moved more at night than during the day. Their home ranges were larger at night, indicating greater activity and further suggesting turtles were taking refuge during the day and foraging at night. Tags used in our study did not provide dive data for the turtles as has been reported in other studies using depth logging tags (e.g., Time Depth Recorders; [78,79,80] or satellite tags that contain depth sensors [81,82,83]. Direct measures of dive behavior from loggerheads would further elucidate these movement patterns, particularly for deep loggerheads [82, 84, 85].

In addition to general movement patterns and diurnal relationships, the loggerheads we tracked exhibited a behavior we termed drifting. These movements occurred both night and day, almost exclusively over deep water and lasted for multiple days. Although not frequently reported, studies have documented a similar surface behavior commonly termed basking however basking is typically short-term (e.g., hours and not days; [79, 86]). Basking has often been suggested as a thermoregulatory action in response to cold temperatures [87, 88] or deep dives below the thermocline [89, 90] which may enhance the digestive process [91, 92]. In our study drifting was more common in colder water temperatures but occurred in all temperatures and seasons (see also [79]), however if there is an optimal temperature at which loggerhead digestive rates are maximized, individuals may exhibit behaviors throughout the year to maintain that optimal temperature regardless of sea surface temperature [92, 93]. For example, in summer when water temperatures rise above 32 °C loggerheads may drift in deeper waters to (1) reduce energy expenditure and (2) move out of warmer, shallow waters [92, 94]. Alternatively in winter, turtles may bask at the surface to increase body temperatures [88].

Home ranges of our tracked loggerheads were small relative to those reported for loggerheads at other sites throughout the world [37, 57, 58, 68, 95], for nesting loggerheads from the Gulf of Mexico [4, 96] and for those previously reported in St. Joseph and St. Andrews Bays, FL [13]. However, location accuracy can greatly impact estimates of home range size [12] and as such, use of GPS technology with small positional error rates relative to Argos-only tags allows the use of a relatively small convolution kernel and likely results in smaller home range estimates [15, 95, 97]. For example, using data from GPS tags [1], documented relatively small home ranges for loggerheads in offshore (90.2 km2) and nearshore (24.3 km2) waters in the Mediterranean. Alternatively, home ranges for loggerheads tracked across the Mediterranean using Argos satellite tags were much larger [32, 95, 98]. We documented no difference in home range sizes relative to movement patterns (i.e., deep vs. shallow-deep). Although the largest home range we documented was a turtle that used deep-water (T.01), the two remaining deep turtles (T.02, T.05) used relatively small home ranges (Table 1). This is in opposition to Dujon et al. [1]. who found offshore home ranges were larger than nearshore home ranges and suggested turtles had to search broader areas in deeper waters for prey. While there were no differences in offshore/nearshore home ranges in St. Joseph Bay, daytime home ranges were much smaller than nighttime home ranges suggesting turtles were more active at night. This is in opposition to diurnal home range sizes reported for green turtles (Christiansen et al. 2017) and hawksbills (Hart et al. 2012, Wood et al. 2017) that suggested turtles rested at night and were active during the day (Hays et al. 2024). Our nocturnal home range sizes support our data that showed some turtles moved onto shoals at night, most likely to forage. Additionally, our tags transmitted a lower rate of satellite communications during the day, further suggesting that turtles exhibited deep water resting behavior during the day resulting in relatively small home ranges and active foraging at night over larger home ranges.

Three of the 10 tracked loggerheads left St. Joseph Bay for approximately 1–2 months when water temperatures dropped in winter. Those three turtles primarily remained in the Gulf of Mexico immediately adjacent to St. Joseph Bay; one undertook a foray of approximately 120 km to the southeast of the bay. Sea turtles inhabiting temperate regions frequently move out of coastal bays in winter to avoid cold temperatures [13, 99, 100]. Individuals that remain in coastal bays risk cold-stunning [101, 102]. Our tagged turtles traveled fastest while in the Gulf of Mexico, which is not surprising as oceanic movements are usually faster than coastal movements [103,104,105]. However, compared to travel speeds reported for loggerheads during migration (e.g., 0.14–0.36 m s− 1 or 0.50–1.30 km h− 1 in [105]), travel speeds for loggerheads outside of St. Joseph Bay (0.0036–1.53 km h− 1) were comparable, while speeds within the bay (0.0036–0.713 km h− 1) were slow [105,106,107]. While in the bay, fastest travel speeds occurred at intermediate water depths, which suggests turtles were traveling between benthic foraging or resting locations and shallow foraging areas [104, 105].

Conclusions

Compared to the more commonly used Argos satellite tags [33, 108], the GPS tags use here were ideal for use on an organism such as a sea turtle that spends little time at the ocean’s surface. The ability of tags to backfill data when turtles are at the surface for long periods of time provides valuable location information that would otherwise be lost to sea turtle tracking studies. In our study, data from two turtles would have been removed due to insufficient number of transmissions, and overall precision from all tags would have declined without the back-filled information.

Our data showed a clear division in habitat use between loggerheads that used only deep waters and those that moved from shallow shoals to deeper waters and highlights the need to look more closely at diving and foraging patterns of these individuals. If these individuals are foraging on separate prey items, there may also be morphological differences, such as variations in head width [109] or body condition [110] between loggerheads that exhibited different movement patterns. Finally, only three of our tracked turtles remained in deep waters throughout the tracking period. This is notable because our capture methods, which require visual location and hand capture of turtles, are only effective in water depths less than approximately 4 m, into which deep turtles rarely move. Continued use of these GPS tags with loggerheads inhabiting coastal foraging areas could confirm whether the movement patterns we documented in St. Joseph Bay occur elsewhere or only at our study site and could help elucidate drivers of these different patterns.

Data availability

Data have been deposited in ScienceBase and can be accessed at https://doi.org/10.5066/P13TCY7X.

References

  1. Dujon AM, Schofield G, Lester RE, Papafitsoros K, Hays GC. Complex movement patterns by foraging loggerhead sea turtles outside the breeding season identified using Argos-linked Fastloc‐Global Positioning System. Mar Ecol. 2018;39:e12489.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Meese EN, Lowe CG. Active acoustic telemetry tracking and tri-axial accelerometers reveal fine-scale movement strategies of a non-obligate ram ventilator. Mov Ecol. 2020;8:8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Nathan R, Getz WM, Revilla E, Holyoak M, Kadmon R, Saltz D, et al. A movement ecology paradigm for unifying organismal movement research. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;105:19052–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Hart KM, Lamont MM, Sartain AR, Fujisaki I. Migration, foraging, and residency patterns for northern Gulf loggerheads: implications of local threats and international movements. PLoS ONE. 2014;9:e103453.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Wright BM, Ford JKB, Ellis GM, Deecke VB, Shapiro AD, Battaile BC, et al. Fine-scale foraging movements by fish-eating killer whales (Orcinus orca) relate to the vertical distributions and escape responses of salmonid prey (Oncorhynchus Spp). Mov Ecol. 2017;5:3.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Heithaus MR, Frid A, Wirsing AJ, Dill LM, Fourqurean JW, Burkholder D, et al. State-dependent risk‐taking by green sea turtles mediates top‐down effects of tiger shark intimidation in a marine ecosystem. J Anim Ecol. 2007;76:837–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Walther BD, Munguia P, Fuiman LA. Frontiers in marine movement ecology: mechanisms and consequences of migration and dispersal in marine habitats. Biol Lett. 2015;11:20150146.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Torney CJ, Hopcraft JGC, Morrison TA, Couzin ID, Levin SA. From single steps to mass migration: the problem of scale in the movement ecology of the Serengeti wildebeest. Phil Trans R Soc B. 2018;373:20170012.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Dodge S, Bohrer G, Bildstein K, Davidson SC, Weinzierl R, Bechard MJ, et al. Environmental drivers of variability in the movement ecology of Turkey vultures (Cathartes aura) in North and South America. Phil Trans R Soc B. 2014;369:20130195.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Van Beest FM, Teilmann J, Dietz R, Galatius A, Mikkelsen L, Stalder D, et al. Environmental drivers of harbour porpoise fine-scale movements. Mar Biol. 2018;165:95.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Lidgard DC, Bowen WD, Iverson SJ. Sex-differences in fine-scale home-range use in an upper-trophic level marine predator. Mov Ecol. 2020;8:11.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Thomson JA, Börger L, Christianen MJA, Esteban N, Laloë J-O, Hays GC. Implications of location accuracy and data volume for home range estimation and fine-scale movement analysis: comparing Argos and Fastloc-GPS tracking data. Mar Biol. 2017;164:204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Lamont M, Iverson A. Shared habitat use by juveniles of three sea turtle species. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2018;606:187–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Griffin LP, Smith BJ, Cherkiss MS, Crowder AG, Pollock CG, Hillis-Starr Z, et al. Space use and relative habitat selection for immature green turtles within a Caribbean marine protected area. Anim Biotelemetry. 2020;8:22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Hays GC, Rattray A, Shimada T, Esteban N. Individual variation in home-range across an ocean basin and links to habitat quality and management. J Appl Ecol. 2024;61:658–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Hart KM, Guzy JC, Smith BJ. Drivers of realized satellite tracking duration in marine turtles. Mov Ecol. 2021;9:1.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Hays GC, Laloë J, Rattray A, Esteban N. Why do Argos satellite tags stop relaying data? Ecol Evo. 2021;11:7093–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Vincent C, Mcconnell BJ, Ridoux V, Fedak MA. Assessment of Argos location accuracy from satellite tags deployed on captive gray seals. Mar Mam Sci. 2002;18:156–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Luschi P, Sözbilen D, Cerritelli G, Ruffier F, Başkale E, Casale P. A biphasic navigational strategy in loggerhead sea turtles. Sci Rep. 2020;10:18130.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Pasanisi E, Chimienti M, Blasi MF, Maffucci F, Hochscheid S. Ocean highways in the western Mediterranean: which are the areas with increased exposure to maritime traffic for loggerhead turtles? Front Mar Sci. 2022;9:924532.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Quattrocchi G, Cucco A, Cerritelli G, Mencacci R, Comparetto G, Sammartano D, et al. Testing a novel aggregated methodology to assess hydrodynamic impacts on a high-resolution marine turtle trajectory. Front Mar Sci. 2021;8:699580.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Schofield G, Bishop CM, Katselidis KA, Dimopoulos P, Pantis JD, Hays GC. Microhabitat selection by sea turtles in a dynamic thermal marine environment. J Anim Ecol. 2009;78:14–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Nowak BVR, Bowen WD, Whoriskey K, Lidgard DC, Mills Flemming JE, Iverson SJ. Foraging behaviour of a continental shelf marine predator, the grey seal (Halichoerus grypus), is associated with < i > in situ < i>, subsurface oceanographic conditions. Mov Ecol. 2020;8:41.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Knip DM, Heupel MR, Simpfendorfer CA. To roam or to home: site fidelity in a tropical coastal shark. Mar Biol. 2012;159:1647–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Panigada S, Donovan GP, Druon J-N, Lauriano G, Pierantonio N, Pirotta E, et al. Satellite tagging of Mediterranean fin whales: working towards the identification of critical habitats and the focussing of mitigation measures. Sci Rep. 2017;7:3365.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Hays GC, Bailey H, Bograd SJ, Bowen WD, Campagna C, Carmichael RH, et al. Translating marine animal tracking data into conservation policy and management. Trends Ecol Ev. 2019;34:459–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Haskell JP, Ritchie ME, Olff H. Fractal geometry predicts varying body size scaling relationships for mammal and bird home ranges. Nature. 2002;418:527–30.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Moffitt EA, Botsford LW, Kaplan DM, O’Farrell MR. Marine reserve networks for species that move within a home range. Ecol App. 2009;19:1835–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Tamburello N, Côté IM, Dulvy NK. Energy and the scaling of animal space use. Am Nat. 2015;186:196–211.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Seminoff J, Resendiz A, Nichols W. Home range of green turtles Chelonia mydas at a coastal foraging area in the Gulf of California, Mexico. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2002;242:253–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Berube MD, Dunbar SG, Rützler K, Hayes WK. Home range and foraging ecology of juvenile hawksbill sea turtles (Eretmochelys imbricata) on inshore reefs of Honduras. Chel Conserv Bio. 2012;11:33–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Abalo-Morla S, Belda EJ, March D, Revuelta O, Cardona L, Giralt S, et al. Assessing the use of marine protected areas by loggerhead sea turtles (Caretta caretta) tracked from the western Mediterranean. Glob Ecol Conserv. 2022;38:e02196.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Hays GC, Hawkes LA. Satellite tracking sea turtles: opportunities and challenges to address key questions. Front Mar Sci. 2018;5:432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Riotte-Lambert L, Matthiopoulos J. Environmental predictability as a cause and consequence of animal movement. Trends Ecol Ev. 2020;35:163–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Fauchald P, Erikstad KE, Skarsfjordi H. Scale-dependent predator-prey interactions: the hierarchical spatial distribution of seabirds and prey. Ecology. 2002;81:773–83.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Lamont MM, Putman NF, Fujisaki I, Hart K. Spatial requirements of different life-stages of the loggerhead turtle (Caretta caretta) from a distinct population segment in the northern Gulf of Mexico. Herp Conserv Biol. 2015;10:26–43.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Shimada T, Jones R, Limpus C, Groom R, Hamann M. Long-term and seasonal patterns of sea turtle home ranges in warm coastal foraging habitats: implications for conservation. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2016;562:163–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Florida Department of Environmental Protection. St. Joseph Bay Aquatic Preserve Management Plan. Tallahassee, FL: Florida Department of Environmental Protection; 2022.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Lamont MM, Fujisaki I, Stephens BS, Hackett C. Home range and habitat use of juvenile green turtles (Chelonia mydas) in the northern Gulf of Mexico. Anim Biotelemetry. 2015;3:53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Jones TT, Van Houtan KS, Bostrom BL, Ostafichuk P, Mikkelsen J, Tezcan E et al. S Rands editor. 2013 Calculating the ecological impacts of animal-borne instruments on aquatic organisms. Methods Ecol Evol 4 1178–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Tomkiewicz SM, Fuller MR, Kie JG, Bates KK. Global positioning system and associated technologies in animal behaviour and ecological research. Phil Trans R Soc B. 2010;365:2163–76.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Dujon AM, Lindstrom RT, Hays GC. The accuracy of Fastloc- GPS locations and implications for animal tracking. Mar Ecol. 2014;5:1162–9.

    Google Scholar 

  43. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing [Internet]. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. 2021. https://www.R-project.org/.

  44. Pebesma E. sf: Simple Features for R [Internet]. 2019. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=sf.

  45. Kay Mggdist. Visualizations of Distributions and Uncertainty [Internet]. 2022. https://mjskay.github.io/ggdist/.

  46. Wickham H. ggplot2: elegant graphics for data analysis. Springer; 2016.

  47. Atmospheric Administration (NOAA). ALLUS80K: medium resolution Digital Vector U.S. Shoreline shapefile. Silver Spring, MD: National Oceanic and. National Ocean Service (NOS), Office of Coast Survey, and the Strategic Environmental assessments. SEA) Division of the Office of Ocean Resources Conservation and Assessment (ORCA); 1994.

  48. Cooperative Institute for Research in Environmental Sciences (CIRES) at the University of Colorado, Boulder. Continuously Updated Digital Elevation Model (CUDEM) – 1/9 Arc-Second Resolution Bathymetric-Topographic Tiles. [Internet]. Silver Spring, MD: NOAA National Centers for Environmental Information. 2014 [cited 2020 Aug 22]. https://doi.org/10.25921/ds9v-ky35.

  49. Baddeley A, Turner R. Spatstat: an R Package for analyzing spatial point patterns. J Stat Softw. 2005;12:1–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using Ime4. J Stat Soft. 2015;67:1–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Bivand R, Lewin-Koh N. maptools: Tools for Handling Spatial Objects [Internet]. 2021. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=maptools.

  52. Benscoter AM, Smith BJ, Hart KM. Loggerhead Marine turtles (Caretta caretta) nesting at smaller sizes than expected in the Gulf of Mexico: implications for turtle behavior, population dynamics, and conservation. Conservat Sci Prac. 2022;4:e581.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. National Marine Fisheries Service and U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service. Recovery plan for the Northwest Atlantic population of the loggerhead sea turtle (Caretta caretta), second rivision. Silver Spring, MD: National Marine Fisheries Service; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  54. Vander Zanden HB, Bjorndal KA, Reich KJ, Bolten AB. Individual specialists in a generalist population: results from a long-term stable isotope series. Biol Lett. 2010;6:711–4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Hawkes LA, Witt MJ, Broderick AC, Coker JW, Coyne MS, Dodd M, et al. Home on the range: spatial ecology of loggerhead turtles in Atlantic waters of the USA. Divers Distrib. 2011;17:624–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Hatase H, Omuta K, Tsukamoto K. A mechanism that maintains alternative life histories in a loggerhead sea turtle population. Ecology. 2013;94:2583–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. González Carman V, Bruno I, Maxwell S, Álvarez K, Albareda D, Acha EM, et al. Habitat use, site fidelity and conservation opportunities for juvenile loggerhead sea turtles in the Río De La Plata, Argentina. Mar Biol. 2016;163:20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Braun McNeill J, Avens L, Goodman Hall A, Fujisaki I, Iverson A. Foraging and overwintering behavior of loggerhead sea turtles Caretta caretta in the western North Atlantic. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2020;641:209–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Jang S-J, Jo K, Jang S, Nishizawa H, Kim M, Balazs G, et al. Connectivity between sea turtles off Jeju Island on the Korean Peninsula, and other populations in the western Pacific. Front Mar Sci. 2024;11:1281897.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Hertel AG, Niemelä PT, Dingemanse NJ, Mueller T. A guide for studying among-individual behavioral variation from movement data in the wild. Mov Ecol. 2020;8:30.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Shaw AK. Causes and consequences of individual variation in animal movement. Mov Ecol. 2020;8:12.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  62. Freitas C, Caldeira R, Reis J, Dellinger T. Foraging behavior of juvenile loggerhead sea turtles in the open ocean: from Lévy exploration to area-restricted search. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2018;595:203–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Chambault P, Dalleau M, Nicet J-B, Mouquet P, Ballorain K, Jean C, et al. Contrasted habitats and individual plasticity drive the fine scale movements of juvenile green turtles in coastal ecosystems. Mov Ecol. 2020;8:1.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Schirmer A, Hoffmann J, Eccard JA, Dammhahn M. My niche: individual spatial niche specialization affects within- and between-species interactions. Proc R Soc B. 2020;287:20192211.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  65. Arends CL, Vander Zanden HB, Lamont MM. Isotopic niche partitioning in a multi-species assemblage. Mar Biol. 2024;171:2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Wallace BP, Avens L, Braun-McNeill J, McClellan CM. The diet composition of immature loggerheads: insights on trophic niche, growth rates, and fisheries interactions. J Exp Mar Bio Ecol. 2009;373:50–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Narazaki T, Sato K, Miyazaki N. Summer migration to temperate foraging habitats and active winter diving of juvenile loggerhead turtles Caretta caretta in the western North Pacific. Mar Biol. 2015;162:1251–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Chimienti M, Blasi MF, Hochscheid S. Movement patterns of large juvenile loggerhead turtles in the Mediterranean Sea: ontogenetic space use in a small ocean basin. Eco Evol. 2020;10:6978–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Narazaki T, Sato K, Abernathy KJ, Marshall GJ, Miyazaki N. Loggerhead Turtles (Caretta caretta) use vision to forage on gelatinous prey in mid-water. Miller PJO, editor. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e66043.

  70. Hart K, White C, Iverson A, Whitney N. Trading shallow safety for deep sleep: juvenile green turtles select deeper resting sites as they grow. Endang Species Res. 2016;31:61–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Lamont MM, Alday J, Alday C. Interspecific interactions among three species of sea turtle using a common resting area. Ecology. 2023;104:e3861.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Silver-Gorges I, Ceriani SA, Fuentes MMPB. Fine-scale intraspecific niche partitioning in a highly mobile, marine megafauna species: implications for ecology and conservation. R Soc Open Sci. 2023;10:221529.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  73. Rooker JR, Dennis GD. Diel, lunar and seasonal changes in a mangrove fish assemblage off southwestern Puerto Rico. Bull Mar Sci. 1991;49:684–98.

    Google Scholar 

  74. Luo J, Serafy J, Sponaugle S, Teare P, Kieckbusch D. Movement of gray snapper Lutjanus griseus among subtropical seagrass, mangrove, and coral reef habitats. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2009;380:255–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Lear KO, Whitney NM, Morris JJ, Gleiss AC. Temporal niche partitioning as a novel mechanism promoting co-existence of sympatric predators in marine systems. Proc R Soc B. 2021;288:20210816.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  76. Christiansen F, Esteban N, Mortimer JA, Dujon AM, Hays GC. Diel and seasonal patterns in activity and home range size of green turtles on their foraging grounds revealed by extended Fastloc-GPS tracking. Mar Biol. 2017;164:10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Furman BT, Peterson BJ, Heck KL. Will the Florida Big Bend area become the next Gulf of Mexico reef tract? Front Mar Sci. 2020;7:334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Hays GC, Metcalfe JD, Walne AW. The implications of lung-regulated buoyancy control for dive depth and duration. Ecology. 2004;85:1137–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Hochscheid S, Bentivegna F, Hamza A, Hays GC. When surfacers do not dive: multiple significance of extended surface times in marine turtles. J Exp Bio. 2010;213:1328–37.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  80. Madrak S, Lewison R, Eguchi T, Klimley A, Seminoff J. Effects of ambient temperature on dive behavior of East Pacific green turtles before and after a power plant closure. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2022;683:157–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  81. Barceló C, Domingo A, Miller P, Ortega L, Giffoni B, Sales G, et al. High-use areas, seasonal movements and dive patterns of juvenile loggerhead sea turtles in the Southwestern Atlantic Ocean. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2013;479:235–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  82. Iverson AR, Fujisaki I, Lamont MM, Hart KM. Loggerhead sea turtle (Caretta caretta) diving changes with productivity, behavioral mode, and sea surface temperature. PLoS ONE. 2019;14:e0220372.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  83. Wildermann NE, Sasso CR, Stokes LW, Snodgrass D, Fuentes MMPB. Habitat Use and Behavior of multiple species of Marine turtles at a foraging area in the Northeastern Gulf of Mexico. Front Mar Sci. 2019;6:155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  84. Hatase H, Omuta K, Tsukamoto K. Bottom or midwater: alternative foraging behaviours in adult female loggerhead sea turtles. J Zool. 2007;273:46–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  85. Kishida N, Okuyama J, Arita M, Kume N, Fujita K, Nishizawa H, et al. A validation of abstracted dive profiles relayed via the Argos satellite system: a case study of a loggerhead turtle. Anim Biotelemetry. 2022;10:21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  86. Schofield G, Katselidis K, Dimopoulos P, Pantis J, Hays G. Behaviour analysis of the loggerhead sea turtle Caretta caretta from direct in-water observation. Endang Species Res. 2006;2:71–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  87. Southwood AL, Andrews RD, Lutcavage ME, Paladino FV, West NH, George RH, et al. Heart rates and diving behavior of leatherback sea turtles in the eastern Pacific Ocean. J Exp Bio. 1999;202:1115–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  88. Lamont MM, Seay DR, Gault K. Overwintering behavior of juvenile sea turtles at a temperate foraging ground. Ecology. 2018;99:2621–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Sakamoto W, Uchida I, Naito Y, Kureha K, Tujimura M, Sato K. Deep diving behavior of the loggerhead turtle near the frontal zone. Nippon Suisan Gakkashi. 1990;56:1435–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  90. Southwood AL, Andrews RD, Paladino FV, Jones DR. Effects of diving and swimming behavior on body temperatures of Pacific leatherback turtles in tropical seas. Physiol Biochem Zool. 2005;78:285–97.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Amorocho DF, Reina RD. Intake passage time, digesta composition and digestibility in East Pacific green turtles (Chelonia mydas agassizii) at Gorgona National Park, Colombian Pacific. J Exp Mar Bio Ecol. 2008;360:117–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  92. Valente AL, Marco I, Parga ML, Lavin S, Alegre F, Cuenca R. Ingesta passage and gastric emptying times in loggerhead sea turtles (Caretta caretta). Res Vet Sci. 2008;84:132–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Wehrle BA, German DP. Reptilian digestive efficiency: past, present, and future. Comp Biochem Physiol Part A. 2023;277:111369.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  94. Lebrasse MC, Schaeffer BA, Coffer MM, Whitman PJ, Zimmerman RC, Hill VJ, et al. Temporal stability of seagrass extent, leaf area, and Carbon storage in St. Joseph Bay, Florida: a semi-automated remote sensing analysis. Estuaries Coast. 2022;45:2082–101.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  95. Schofield G, Hobson VJ, Fossette S, Lilley MKS, Katselidis KA, Hays GC. Fidelity to foraging sites, consistency of migration routes and habitat modulation of home range by sea turtles. Divers Distrib. 2010;16:840–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  96. Foley AM, Schroeder BA, Hardy R, MacPherson SL, Nicholas M. Long-term behavior at foraging sites of adult female loggerhead sea turtles (Caretta caretta) from three Florida rookeries. Mar Biol. 2014;161:1251–62.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  97. Witt MJ, Åkesson S, Broderick AC, Coyne MS, Ellick J, Formia A, et al. Assessing accuracy and utility of satellite-tracking data using Argos-linked Fastloc-GPS. Anim Behav. 2010;80:571–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  98. Zbinden JA, Aebischer A, Margaritoulis D, Arlettaz R. Important areas at sea for adult loggerhead sea turtles in the Mediterranean Sea: satellite tracking corroborates findings from potentially biased sources. Mar Biol. 2008;153:899–906.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  99. Seney E, Landry A. Movement patterns of immature and adult female Kemp’s Ridley sea turtles in the northwestern Gulf of Mexico. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2011;440:241–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  100. Metz TL, Gordon M, Mokrech M, Guillen G. Movements of juvenile green turtles (Chelonia mydas) in the nearshore waters of the northwestern Gulf of Mexico. Front Mar Sci. 2020;7:647.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  101. Avens L, Goshe L, Harms C, Anderson E, Goodman Hall A, Cluse W, et al. Population characteristics, age structure, and growth dynamics of neritic juvenile green turtles in the northeastern Gulf of Mexico. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2012;458:213–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  102. Mollenhauer R, Lamont MM, Foley A. Long-term apparent survival of a cold‐stunned subpopulation of juvenile green turtles. Ecosphere. 2022;13:e4221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  103. Hart K, Fujisaki I. Satellite tracking reveals habitat use by juvenile green sea turtles Chelonia mydas in the Everglades, Florida, USA. Endang Species Res. 2010;11:221–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  104. Dujon AM, Schofield G, Lester RE, Esteban N, Hays GC. Fastloc-GPS reveals daytime departure and arrival during long-distance migration and the use of different resting strategies in sea turtles. Mar Biol. 2017;164:187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  105. Kinoshita C, Fukuoka T, Narazaki T, Niizuma Y, Sato K. Analysis of why sea turtles swim slowly: a metabolic and mechanical approach. J Exp Bio. 2021;224:jeb236216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  106. Abecassis M, Senina I, Lehodey P, Gaspar P, Parker D, Balazs G et al. A model of loggerhead sea turtle (Caretta caretta) habitat and movement in the oceanic North Pacific. Hays GC, editor. PLoS ONE. 2013;8:e73274.

  107. Narazaki T, Sato K, Abernathy KJ, Marshall GJ, Miyazaki N. Sea turtles compensate deflection of heading at the sea surface during directional travel. J Exp Bio. 2009;212:4019–26.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  108. Jeffers VF, Godley BJ. Satellite tracking in sea turtles: how do we find our way to the conservation dividends? Biol Conserv. 2016;199:172–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  109. Price JT, Pfaller JB, Vander Zanden HB, Williams KL, Bolten AB, Bjorndal KA. Foraging area, not trophic position, is linked to head size variation in adult female loggerhead turtles. J Zool. 2017;302:279–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  110. Lamont MM, Johnson D. Variation in species composition, size and fitness of two multi-species sea turtle assemblages using different neritic habitats. Front Mar Sci. 2021;7:608740.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  111. Lamont MM, Slone DH, Alday J, Reid JP, Butler SM, Arends CL. et al. Loggerhead Iridium Locations in Northwest Florida 2019-2021: U.S. Geological Survey data release. ScienceBase. 2024. https://doi.org/10.5066/P13TCY7X.

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to Carson Arends, Samantha Snow and Daniel Catizone for field support. We thank Hardin Waddle for his review.

Funding

The authors have no funding to declare.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

ML and DS acquired resources for this project. All authors designed the study and conducted field investigations. ML and DS conceptualized manuscript objectives. DS analyzed data. ML led writing of the manuscript. All authors contributed to revisions and final submission approval.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Margaret M. Lamont.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval

All turtle handling and sampling was conducted under State of Florida Marine Turtle Permits 032, 118 and 152 and National Marine Fisheries Service permit 21366, and performed according to the Institutional Animal Care Protocol USGS-SESC-IACUC-2019-15.

Disclaimer

Any use of trade, firm, or product names is for descriptive purposes only and does not imply endorsement by the US Government.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic Supplementary Material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary Material 1

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lamont, M.M., Slone, D., Reid, J. et al. Deep vs shallow: GPS tags reveal a dichotomy in movement patterns of loggerhead turtles foraging in a coastal bay. Mov Ecol 12, 40 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40462-024-00480-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40462-024-00480-y

Keywords