Skip to main content

Accelerometer informed time-energy budgets reveal the importance of temperature to the activity of a wild, arid zone canid

Abstract

Background

Globally, arid regions are expanding and becoming hotter and drier with climate change. For medium and large bodied endotherms in the arid zone, the necessity to dissipate heat drives a range of adaptations, from behaviour to anatomy and physiology. Understanding how apex predators negotiate these landscapes and how they balance their energy is important as it may have broad impacts on ecosystem function.

Methods

We used tri-axial accelerometry (ACC) and GPS data collected from free-ranging dingoes in central Australia to investigate their activity-specific energetics, and activity patterns through time and space. We classified dingo activity into stationary, walking, and running behaviours, and estimated daily energy expenditure via activity-specific time-energy budgets developed using energy expenditure data derived from the literature. We tested whether dingoes behaviourally thermoregulate by modelling ODBA as a function of ambient temperature during the day and night. We used traditional distance measurements (GPS) as well as fine-scale activity (ODBA) data to assess their daily movement patterns.

Results

We retrieved ACC and GPS data from seven dingoes. Their mass-specific daily energy expenditure was significantly lower in summer (288 kJ kg− 1 day− 1) than winter (495 kJ kg− 1 day− 1; p = 0.03). Overall, dingoes were much less active during summer where 91% of their day was spent stationary in contrast to just 46% during winter. There was a sharp decrease in ODBA with increasing ambient temperature during the day (R2 = 0.59), whereas ODBA increased with increasing Ta at night (R2 = 0.39). Distance and ODBA were positively correlated (R = 0.65) and produced similar crepuscular patterns of activity.

Conclusion

Our results indicate that ambient temperature may drive the behaviour of dingoes. Seasonal differences of daily energy expenditure in free-ranging eutherian mammals have been found in several species, though this was the first time it has been observed in a wild canid. We conclude that the negative relationship between dingo activity (ODBA) and ambient temperature during the day implies that high heat gain from solar radiation may be a factor limiting diurnal dingo activity in an arid environment.

Introduction

Movement is the primary contributor to active energy expenditure in most vertebrates [1,2,3]. Animals move to improve their individual fitness through, for example, access to food resources, to avoid predators, or to find mates. Underlying these behaviours is the need to balance energy acquisition and expenditure, which ultimately determines an animal’s behaviour and location in the landscape [4,5,6]. In addition, variation in the landscape structure such as substrate, vegetation type, and elevation will have varying movement costs [7, 8]. Given that animals tend not to position themselves randomly [5, 9,10,11], understanding purposive movements and use of space provides insight into the ecophysiology of mobile taxa.

The presence of medium and large carnivores in a landscape can strongly influence the structure and function of ecosystems [12,13,14]. In fact, mammalian predators are often used as bio-indicators in the event of human-induced ecosystem disruption given their local extinction can trigger a trophic cascade [15]. Quantifying the behaviour and resulting energy demands of free-ranging carnivores is therefore useful for predicting their resource requirements and subsequent selection of patchily distributed resources across the landscape.

Australia’s largest terrestrial predator, the dingo Canis dingo, is a medium-sized eutherian carnivore that persists in a wide range of environments [16]. Dingoes are a highly mobile species that traverse large areas to acquire resources, and maintain social ties and territorial boundaries. As a result, the decision to move is biologically significant and likely to vary at fine (e.g., daily) and broad (e.g., seasonal) temporal scales, as well as spatially. For populations in the harsh, resource-limited deserts of central Australia where risks of hyperthermia are high, survival depends on making choices that minimise behavioural energetic expenditure (and evaporative water loss), whilst optimising the acquisition of food, shelter, and water resources needed for survival [17].

How wild animals balance their energetics through time and space is increasingly being studied by integrating movement data with activity-specific time-energy budgets [18, 19]. Animal movement can be reliably captured by animal-attached accelerometers (ACC) that measure changes in acceleration in up to three axes [20]. As energy expenditure is a function of activity, behaviours can then be linked to activity specific measures of energy expenditure to produce robust time-energy budgets in free-ranging animals. Time-energy budgets, the categorisation of energy cost per activity integrated over the time spent performing that activity, provide a reliable estimate of daily energy expenditure [21]. Measuring energetic costs for free-ranging and highly mobile predators like dingoes is challenging and, to date, we have a limited understanding of how physiological capacities and environmental variables affect their movement and use of space.

It has been suggested that an individual’s maximal energy expenditure may be constrained by their ability to dissipate heat [22] and therefore bodily processes that generate heat (e.g., movement, digestion etc.) trade-off within a total limit defined by heat dissipation capacity. For large (> 10 kg) mammals in arid regions, the ability to lose heat is limited by low surface area to volume ratios and thus cooling can be slow [23]. This is increasingly important to understand in the wake of global climate change as arid regions are likely to experience even hotter temperatures and prolonged droughts [24]. As a first response, individuals are most likely to adjust their short term behaviour before longer term physiological adaptations or range adjustments [23].

Investigating how dingoes behave in an already challenging environment could potentially provide us insights into how other arid zone predators cope under climate change. Here, we used ACC and GPS data collected from free-ranging dingoes in central Australia to investigate their behaviour-specific energetics and activity patterns through time and space, and uncover the trade-offs imposed by their arid habitat. We classified broad classes of behaviour from ACC data and used it to estimate daily energy expenditure via activity-specific time-energy budgets. We explored the dingo’s behaviour at different times of the day and year, and examined daily patterns of activity in response to ambient temperature as an indication of behavioural thermoregulation. We expected that dingoes would be most active at night and that during periods of high temperature dingoes would be inactive to reduce the risk of hyperthermia. Finally, we explored how dingoes behave and partition their energy in relation to landscape features.

Methods

Study area and species

Our study took place from April 2016 to May 2018 at Kalamurina Wildlife Sanctuary (hereafter ‘Kalamurina’), a 6670 km2 conservation area owned and managed by Australian Wildlife Conservancy, and located at the intersection of three of Australia’s central deserts: the Simpson, Tirari, and Sturt’s Stony Desert (27°48’S, 137°40’E, UTM Zone 54S; Fig. 1). The site adjoins protected areas to the north and south to create a 64,064 km2 contiguous area that is managed for conservation. The region’s climate is arid with a median annual rainfall of 133.5 mm, and it is characterised by very hot summers and mild winters; mean daily temperatures ranging from 23 °C – 38 °C in the hottest month and 6 °C – 20 °C in the coldest month (with mid-afternoon being the hottest part of the day) [25]. It is located in the Simpson-Strzelecki Dunes Bioregion and the dominant landform is sand dunes (< 18 m), with scattered floodplains, claypans, and salt lakes. The dune swales are characterised by chenopod shrubland where the main vegetation are species of Acacia, Eremophila, and Atriplex. Extensive coolabah Eucalyptus coolabah woodlands exist along the banks and floodplains of the larger watercourses.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Tracking data from seven dingoes at Kalamurina Sanctuary. Inset displays the location of Kalamurina in central Australia. The Warburton Creek is the only major watercourse on the eastern side of the study site, and it is bordered by shrubland and desert woodland along its length. The majority of Kalamurina consists of sand dunes and flats

The dingoes at Kalamurina possess high levels of dingo ancestry [26], making this study the first assessment of energetics in a wild population of pure dingoes. European rabbits Oryctolagus cuniculus, c. 1.6 kg comprise the bulk of their diet, but they also consume reptiles, birds, invertebrates, and vegetation [27].

Data collection, cleaning, and processing

Dingoes were captured using Victor Soft Catch® #3 leg-hold traps modified with Paws-I-Trip pans and a Jake Chain Rig (Professional Trapp Supplies, Molendinar, Queensland). These traps and modifications are designed to reduce the impact on the trapped limb [28]. All traps were set within close proximity (< 20 m) to tracks and checked twice daily within 3 h of sunrise and sunset. All methodology employed as part of this study were ethically reviewed and approved (University of Adelaide Animal Ethics Committee S-2015-177A). We fit 19 dingoes with ACC-GPS collars (Telemetry Solutions, Concord, CA, USA) that were equipped with tri-axial accelerometers (LISD2H, ST Microelectronics, USA) programmed to sample changes in acceleration at 1 Hz (one sample per second) and orientated so that the x, y, and z-axes recorded acceleration along the sway, heave, and surge planes, respectively. To increase the temporal window of data collection, accelerometers were scheduled to record on a one-day on, three-days off sampling regime. We programmed the GPS to record a location every 15 min on the days the ACC was active. Six collars were recovered via triggering the drop-off mechanism, and one was recovered by re-trapping.

To limit the effect of abnormal behaviour that might occur as a result of capture and collaring, we discarded any GPS and ACC data recorded during the 24 h immediately following release. Data were also discarded if they had a horizontal dilution of precision ≥9 (a measure of GPS accuracy) or occurred after the collar had dropped-off. We were able to retrieve ACC and GPS data from seven dingoes. Three individuals were tracked during winter 2016 and four during summer 2017–2018. All data manipulation and analyses were conducted in the R software environment for statistical and graphical computing (version 3.5.1 [29];).

Dingo behaviour, ODBA, and energetic expenditure

We classified wild dingo behaviours from the ACC data using the Random Forest model described in Tatler et al. [30]. This supervised-learning approach required ACC data to be manually classified into behaviours, which was achieved by observing captive dingoes (n = 3) that were equipped with tri-axial ACC units programmed to sample at 1 Hz. The predictive performance of the model was assessed using out-of-sample validation and resulted in the accurate classification of 14 dingo behaviours. However, for the purposes of this paper, we were only interested in general movement patterns that would influence daily energy expenditure. Therefore, we trained a new Random Forest model (with the same set of parameters as in 30) to identify five classes of movement: lying down, sitting, standing, walking, and running (Table 1). Grouping the raw ACC data from the highest and lowest intensity behaviours increased the sample size and improved the accuracy at which our model classified these movements (Table 1). Once we had classified our wild dingo ACC data into five behaviours, we relabelled Lying, Standing, and Sitting behaviours to ‘Stationary’ as these behaviours are so similar that they are unlikely to differ energetically.

Table 1 Performance of the Random Forest model at predicting 14 different behaviours versus grouped behaviours (from Tatler et al [30];). We combined similar behaviours to create three broad movement classes. The True Skill Statistic was used as our measure of classification accuracy, and the 95% confidence intervals are presented in square brackets next to each metric

The total acceleration recorded by accelerometers is the result of both static (gravitational) and dynamic (animal movement) components. Overall dynamic body acceleration uses the dynamic component and thus acceleration due to gravity must be removed. We calculated dynamic body acceleration (DBA) by subtracting a running mean (five seconds) from each acceleration axis (x, y, and z) to give acceleration values occurring from movement. The absolute value of DBA for each axis was then summed to give a per-second value of ODBA. A similar metric also derived from DBA, vectoral DBA (VeDBA), has also been shown to accurately predict energy expenditure for different behaviours in wild animals. Given Tatler et al. [30] found ODBA to be a better predictor of dingo behaviour than VeDBA, and that ODBA and VeDBA do not differ significantly in their ability to predict energy expenditure [19], we chose to estimate energy expenditure in dingoes using ODBA.

Energy calculation

Time-energy budgets have been shown to be an effective estimate of daily energy expenditure when compared to doubly labelled water in previous trials (Weathers et al., 1984) and more recently ODBA was shown to accurately predict energy expenditure for specific activities [31]. We calculated daily energy expenditure using time-energy budgets calculated from our ACC derived behaviours and equations derived from the literature. For resting metabolic rate (applied to all stationary behaviours) we used oxygen consumption data from dingoes collected by Shield [32] and derived the following equation (Eq. 1) for V̇O2 against Ta.

$$ \dot{V}{O}_2\ \left( ml\ {kg}^{-1}{\mathit{\min}}^{-1}\right)=0.007\times {T_a}^2-0.298\times {T}_a+9.968 $$
(1)

Where Ta was calculated per second using the Env-DATA system on Movebank (see ‘Environmental covariates’ section below). Shield [33] calculated V̇O2 using flow through respirometry from dingoes of a similar size to those in our study (mean ± se = 18.8 ± 0.2 kg vs 18.1 ± 0.4 kg) and respirometry was conducted across a similar temperature range to that experienced by the dingoes at Kalamurina. We selected V̇O2 data from the control group in Shield [33] as they were kept in an average ambient temperature of 23 °C over the course of their study, which was not distinctly different from the average ambient temperature at Kalamurina over our study period (26 ± 0.1 °C). For the purpose of this study, it was assumed that the rate of energy expenditure when sleeping is the same as when stationary as metabolic rate has not been measured in sleeping dingoes and we did not differentiate sleeping behaviour within our behavioural classifications. As such our calculations of daily energy expenditure may be slightly overestimated. For our walking and running behaviours we calculated energy expenditure using the following equation (Eq. 2) from Bryce and Williams [34] assuming an average speed of 1.985 m s− 1 for walking and 4.96 m s− 1 for running.

$$ \dot{V}{O}_2\ \left( ml\ {kg}^{-1}{\mathit{\min}}^{-1}\right)=7.5+6.16\times speed $$
(2)

We selected the ‘northern breed’ complex of domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) as classified in Bryce and Williams [34] as these breeds most closely resemble dingoes in overall body size conditions and unfortunately, to the best of our knowledge, no data exist for V̇O2 of active dingoes. We did not account for ambient temperature during active behaviours because this is unlikely to have an additive effect on energy expenditure at low temperatures as heat generated from movement is often substituted for thermoregulation. Yet we cannot account for any additive effect ambient temperature may have on energy expenditure during activity in hyperthermic conditions. This is an entirely unstudied aspect of exercise physiology, with data only reported for a single individual primate [35]. Total daily energy expenditure was calculated per day for each individual by summing the cost of each activity multiplied by the time (in hours) each activity was undertaken. This was then converted to kJ kg− 1 day− 1 by multiplying by a factor of 20.1 [36].

Environmental covariates

We created a map of the major landscape features in the study area using vegetation data from NatureMaps [37] and a spatial layer representing tracks and permanent water sources on Kalamurina provided by the Australian Wildlife Conservancy. We identified seven landscape features from the GIS data; watercourses, desert woodland, low shrubland, tracks, salt lakes, sand dunes, and flats. Landscape features provided varying amounts of shade based on vegetative cover, from full shade (desert woodland) to completely exposed (salt lakes). Each landscape layer was rasterized to the same resolution (25 m) and extent (56,366, 6,798,279; 339,166, 7,094,179; UTM Zone 54S) using the R package ‘raster’ [38]. Additional raster layers were generated from the landscape rasters by calculating the shortest distance from every cell to each landscape feature. Prior to statistical analysis, all such ‘distance to landscape feature’ variables were standardised (x – mean (x) / standard deviation (x)) and pairwise correlations (Pearson’s r) were calculated. Distance to flats was removed from the analyses because it was highly correlated with distance to sand dunes (r = 0.84). All other pairwise correlations were low (r < 0.7).

We used the Env-DATA system on Movebank to annotate environmental data (temperature, NDVI, rainfall, and wind speed) to each GPS location, with information sourced from the European Centre for Medium-Range Weather Forecasts [39] and NASA Land Processes Distributed Active Archive Center [40]. We collected data in two different field seasons, ‘winter’: April–August 2016, and ‘summer’ Oct 2017 – Jan 2018, and used the R library ‘Maptools’ [41] to calculate astronomical time of day (day, night, dawn, and dusk). We then grouped dawn and dusk together as ‘twilight’. We extracted hour and Julian day from our dataset as additional temporal covariates.

Identification of high-use ‘shelters’

Dingoes repeatedly shelter in discrete areas for resting, rearing offspring, and/or socialising (hereafter referred to as ‘shelters’) and thus they may be an important predictor of energy use [42]. We used the R package ‘recurse’ [43] to identify shelters for each dingo by using a combination of 1) revisiting the same location (25 m radius), and 2) the average amount of time spent at that location (residence time). Shelters were defined individually for each dingo by an average residence time per visit of ≥60 min and a rate of recursion in the 90th percentile of all recursions, i.e., the highest rate of revisitation (Table 2).

Table 2 Attributes of the seven dingoes equipped with ACC-GPS collars at Kalamurina including the number of shelters, the mean (± se) daily distance travelled (km d-1), and daily energy expenditure (kJ kg-1 d-1)

Statistical analysis

Behaviour in space and time

Dingoes may exhibit different behavioural responses depending on their location in the landscape. So that individual differences in behaviour through space and time could be clearly identified, we chose to analyse the relationship between behaviour and landscape features for each dingo separately, using a multinomial logistic regression in the R package ‘MDM’ [44]. Our dependent variable was the proportion of time a dingo was engaged in each behaviour (stationary, walking, and running) in the 900 s (i.e., 15 min) prior to the GPS fix, with landscape feature as our predictor variable. To investigate population-level seasonal differences in dingo behaviour, we performed a meta-analysis to generate global parameter estimates across dingoes tracked during winter (JT04, JT05, and JT07) and summer (JT32, JT34, JT36, and JT37). We weighted the estimates from the individual models by the inverse of each estimate’s standard error, to account for variation in the sample size.

Daily activity

To investigate daily activity patterns of dingoes at Kalamurina we constructed two generalised additive models (GAMs) using the R package ‘mgcv’ [45]. Our first activity model assessed movement distance (between successive 15-min GPS locations) as a function of hour of day (0–23). Similarly, the second activity model assessed ODBA (averaged across the same, preceding 15-min period as the distance measure) as a function of hour. Prior to statistical analysis, both response variables were standardised (x – mean (x) / standard deviation (x)). GAMs were fitted with a cyclic cubic regression spline and 20 knots. We also ran a Pearson’s correlation to test the strength and direction of the correlation between the response variables.

To assess whether dingoes exhibited behavioural thermoregulation by adjusting their activity levels as a result of ambient temperature, we used a generalised linear model (GLM) with ODBA (standardised) as our response variable, and ambient temperature (standardised) and time of day (day or night) as our predictors. We included dingo ‘ID’ and ‘Julian day’ as random effects.

Effect of landscape features on behaviour

We used linear mixed effect models in the R package ‘lme4’ [46] to explore the relationship between (log transformed) ODBA values (using 5 s average around each GPS timestamp) and our environmental/temporal covariates. Based on our aims and a priori assumptions of dingo activity, we built a candidate set of models (n = 25) and used Akaike’s Information Criterion corrected for small sample sizes (AICc) and conditional R2 (R package ‘MuMin’ [47];) to rank the models. All candidate models included dingo ID as a random effect to account for individual variation.

Results

Dingoes were much less active during summer where 91 ± 0.04% (mean ± sd) of their day (24 h) was spent stationary versus only 46 ± 0.1% during winter (Table S1). Season had the most profound effect on dingo behaviour (Fig. 2). In summer, dingoes were much more likely to remain stationary than any other behaviour, regardless of where they were in the landscape. In contrast, dingoes were just as likely to be stationary as they were to be walking or running during winter. The model with the lowest AICc and highest R2 (0.57) nested behaviour within ID, included Julian day as a random effect, and landscape feature, time of day, and the interaction between time of day and season as fixed effects.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Dingoes were much less active during summer than they were in winter. Predicted probabilities of being stationary, walking, or running in each habitat. Blue represents dingoes tracked during winter (n = 3), and red represents dingoes tracked during summer (n = 4). Hollow circles indicate probabilities for individual dingoes and solid circles represent global estimates (± 95% confidence intervals) from our meta-analysis of the multinomial model estimates for each individual. In winter, only one dingo occurred on salt lakes, so no global statistic was calculated

Distance and ODBA were positively correlated with each other (r = 0.65, p < 0.001), and both variables indicated crepuscular patterns of activity (Fig. 3). Dingoes were most active at dawn and into the early hours of the night and least active just before dawn and in the middle of the afternoon when compared across the entire study period. However, during winter dingoes were significantly less active during twilight but more active at night than dingoes in summer. The overall activity level of dingoes during the day was not significantly different between summer and winter.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Distance and ODBA were positively correlated with each other (r = 0.65, p < 0.001), and both of these variables indicated crepuscular patterns of activity. Daily activity patterns of dingoes (n = 7) at Kalamurina. The blue dotted line represents the distance moved between successive 15 min GPS location, and the solid green line represents the predicted, mean ODBA value across 900 s (i.e., 15 min), as a function of hour of day. The axis for ODBA has been scaled to fit within the range of the predicted distance values

We found a contrasting relationship between ODBA and Ta that was driven by the time of day (i.e. whether it was day or night; Fig. 4). There was a sharp decrease in ODBA with increasing Ta during the day (R2 = 0.59), whereas ODBA increased with increasing Ta at night (R2 = 0.39). Estimates of mean daily energy expenditure are shown in Table 2. The mean estimated energy expenditure of dingoes was significantly higher in winter (495 kJ kg− 1 day− 1) than summer (288 kJ kg− 1 day− 1; p = 0.03).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Predicted ODBA values (activity) by ambient temperature for dingoes (n = 7) at Kalamurina either during the day (Panel a) or at night (Panel b). The 95% confidence intervals are represented by grey shading

There was a significant effect of landscape feature on the activity levels (ODBA) of dingoes at Kalamurina (Fig. 5). Dingoes were most active on salt lakes, tracks, and flats, and least active when at their shelters (Table S2). However, the time of day had a significant effect on how active dingoes were in each landscape feature during summer but not in winter (Fig. 5). Overall, dingoes exhibited a moderate - low level of activity in most landscape features.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Predicted ODBA values from our selected generalised linear mixed-effect model for dingoes (n = 7) in eight landscape features during the day, night, and twilight. Approximate activity levels (low, moderate, and high) were adapted from the relationship between ODBA and behaviour reported in Tatler et al. [30] and broadly represent our three grouped behaviour classes (stationary, walking, and running), respectively. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals

Discussion

Patterns and processes of all life in the arid zone are shaped by extremes in temperature and water availability. Kalamurina is one of the hottest and driest places in Australia with a long term median annual rainfall < 135 mm and maximum temperatures regularly above 40 °C throughout summer. This region is predicted to experience an increase in heat related extremes and duration of warm spells which could triple the number of days above 40 °C by 2090 [24]. In our study the activity of arid zone dingoes, as measured by ODBA, was primarily driven by ambient temperature. More specifically, we found that this was reflected in activity patterns across time of day, season, and even landscape features. Our results suggest that under future climate scenarios dingoes may shift their behaviour to avoid hyperthermia.

Dingoes in this study were largely crepuscular, with two troughs in activity occurring during the early hours of the morning and the hottest part of the day (mid-afternoon). Akin to other animals, the movement ecology of dingoes is influenced by seasonally-variable intrinsic and extrinsic factors, with either primarily diurnal or primarily nocturnal activity patterns reported in other studies [42, 48]. Activity patterns of predators usually coincide with those of their major food source, which are also linked to ambient temperature [49, 50]. Rabbits comprise the bulk of the dingo’s diet in the arid zone [27], with most rabbit activity occurring at night regardless of season [51]. The time of day and seasonal differences in the activity of dingoes in this study is therefore unlikely to be driven solely by prey acquisition. Moreover, as a vagile species, constraining high activity movements (or reducing them altogether) to the less climatically extreme times of the day is likely a behavioural adaptation to mitigate thermal stressors associated with desert life [5, 52, 53]. While we were unable to track the activity of the same individuals across seasons, different patterns of daily movement during summer and winter have been shown to occur within individual dingoes in this study system [42]. This suggests that our findings are more likely a response to temperature variation between seasons and not simply an artifact of differences between individuals.

Seasonally driven activity constraints have been reported for other species (e.g., flying squirrels [54], and desert woodrats [55]) and suggests a trade-off between remaining in areas which offer thermal respite versus obtaining resources. Seasonal differences in daily energy expenditure in free-ranging eutherian mammals have been found in several species, though this is the first time it has been observed in a wild canid. It has been shown that dingoes are capable of acclimating physiologically to extreme temperatures (− 41 °C to + 45 °C) over the course of a few months by reducing their metabolic rate [33]. Warm acclimated individuals exposed to temperatures up to 45 °C reduced their metabolic rate by around 40% compared to control individuals. This was observed in concert with a change in thermal conductance, an important component of heat dissipation, brought about by altered coat composition [33]. It is reasonable to assume that these physiological changes could contribute to seasonal changes in daily energy expenditure for dingoes in our study, however behavioural thermoregulation via altered activity patterns remains an important facet of energy balancing exhibited by dingoes.

Movement is energetically costly and evaporative water loss is highest during energetically demanding activities at high ambient temperatures [1, 56]. During locomotion even at low ambient temperatures (< 10 °C) canines can rapidly reach high body temperatures (15–20 minutes to reach 42 °C), due to the heat produced by muscle activity [57]. Therefore, at high ambient temperatures the additive heat produced during activity must be actively dumped, potentially further increasing energetic costs. As such dingoes would benefit from remaining inactive in the heat in order to reduce hyperthermia and evaporative water loss. We found that dingoes were stationary for approximately 22 h a day during summer compared to only 12 h during winter. Winter also coincided with the breeding and whelping seasons, which could also explain why dingoes were more active during this time (e.g., searching for mates). Further, the daily energy expenditure of the two female dingoes tracked in winter was considerably higher than the male’s, which may be a consequence of increased metabolic demands associated with lactation. Activity levels of lactating females rise in response to increased foraging effort due to additional energetic demands and fluid requirements for milk production, which can be twice those of basal needs [58]. However, as we were unable to make direct observations of reproductive status this is merely speculative.

Dingoes displayed the highest activity levels on salt lakes and tracks, which was expected given they are primarily used for commuting [42]. These exposed parts of the landscape are likely used as directional travel routes between resources such as water, shelter, and food but are also important for communication as dingoes mark their territory by depositing visual and olfactory cues (e.g., faeces and urine) in conspicuous places to maximise their detection by conspecifics. Returning to discrete areas for shelter and/or denning is common amongst mammalian carnivores and can increase individual fitness by providing thermoregulatory benefits [59], reducing predation rates [60], and increasing offspring survival rates [61]. Further, microclimate selection (i.e., location of shelters in the landscape) is an important thermal defence employed by animals to buffer changes in ambient temperature. We previously collected data on the same dingo population and found that shelters were significantly more likely to be located in the densely vegetated desert woodlands and along watercourses than in exposed habitats like salt lakes [42].

Regardless of season, we found evidence that dingoes behaviourally thermoregulate by decreasing their activity levels with increasing ambient temperature during the day. Conversely, the positive relationship between activity and temperature at night suggests that dingoes could be compensating for low daily activity by partially shifting their movement to nocturnal periods to avoid solar radiation. As the most energetically costly behaviours for dingoes occur in exposed landscapes, shifting movements to the night would mitigate the issue of radiative heat gain while still enabling dingoes to perform important behaviours, such as hunting and socialising.

Conclusions

Understanding the flexibility of behavioural thermoregulation in the arid zone informs our understanding of how populations or species will respond to a changing climate. Here, we suggest that the behavioural ecology of a medium sized carnivore in the arid zone is driven by limitations to heat dissipation regardless of season, in line with the hypothesis that heat dissipation limits the upper boundary of total energy expenditure [22]. The shifts in behaviour observed in this study in response to increasing ambient temperatures have also been reported for other arid zone carnivores, such as Namibian cheetahs (Acinonyx jubatus) and African wild-dogs (Lycaon pictus) [62, 63]. Given the previously reported physiological capacity of dingoes to acclimate to temperatures exceeding 40 °C within months [33], alongside evidence of behavioural thermoregulation reported here, it appears that dingoes may be equipped to survive a predicted increase in temperatures in their environment, albeit via behavioural shifts. This may reflect how other apex predators in arid environments will respond to climate change and could have significant repercussions for predator-prey dynamics and intraguild competition in these ecosystems.

Availability of data and materials

Data found in this manuscript is made available via Figshare: https://doi.org/10.25909/5c901de204d18

References

  1. Schmidt-Nielsen K. Locomotion: energy costs of swimming, flying, and running. Science. 1972;177(4045):222–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Tatner P, Bryant DM. Flight cost of a small passerine measured using doubly labeled water: Implications for energetics studies. Auk. 1986;103(1):169–80. https://doi.org/10.1093/auk/103.1.169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Karasov WH. Daily energy expenditure and the cost of activity in mammals. Am Zool. 2015;32(2):238–48. https://doi.org/10.1093/icb/32.2.238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Harding KC, Fujiwara M, Axberg Y, Härkönen T. Mass-dependent energetics and survival in harbour seal pups. Funct Ecol. 2005;19(1):129–35. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0269-8463.2005.00945.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Nathan R, Getz WM, Revilla E, Holyoak M, Kadmon R, Saltz D, et al. A movement ecology paradigm for unifying organismal movement research. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105(49):19052–9. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0800375105.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Wilson RP, Quintana F, Hobson VJ. Construction of energy landscapes can clarify the movement and distribution of foraging animals. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2012;279(1730):975–80. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2011.1544.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Rubenson J, Henry HT, Dimoulas PM, Marsh RL. The cost of running uphill: Linking organismal and muscle energy use in guinea fowl (numida meleagris). J Exp Biol. 2006;209(13):2395–408. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.02310.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Wall J, Douglas-Hamilton I, Vollrath F. Elephants avoid costly mountaineering. Curr Biol. 2006;16(14):R527–R9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2006.06.049.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Wolf JBW, Kauermann G, Trillmich F. Males in the shade: Habitat use and sexual segregation in the galápagos sea lion (zalophus californianus wollebaeki). Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2005;59(2):293–302. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-005-0042-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Revilla E, Wiegand T. Individual movement behavior, matrix heterogeneity, and the dynamics of spatially structured populations. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105(49):19120–5. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0801725105.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Wilson RP. Resource partitioning and niche hyper-volume overlap in free-living pygoscelid penguins. Funct Ecol. 2010;24(3):646–57. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2435.2009.01654.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Estes JA, Terborgh J, Brashares JS, Power ME, Berger J, Bond WJ, et al. Trophic downgrading of planet earth. Science. 2011;333(6040):301–6. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1205106.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Ritchie EG, Johnson CN. Predator interactions, mesopredator release and biodiversity conservation. Ecol Lett. 2009;12(9):982–98. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1461-0248.2009.01347.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Soulé ME, Estes JA, Berger J, Del Rio CM. Ecological effectiveness: Conservation goals for interactive species. Conserv Biol. 2003;17(5):1238–50. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1523-1739.2003.01599.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Berger J, Stacey PB, Bellis L, Johnson MP. A mammalian predator–prey imbalance: Grizzly bear and wolf extinction affect avian neotropical migrants. Ecol Appl. 2001;11(4):947–60. https://doi.org/10.1890/1051-0761(2001)011[0947:AMPPIG]2.0.CO;2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Fleming P, Corbett L, Harden B, Thomson P. In: Bomford M, editor. Managing the impact of dingoes and other wild dogs. Canberra: Bureau of Rural Sciences; 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Allen BL. Do desert dingoes drink daily? Visitation rates at remote waterpoints in the strzelecki desert. Aust Mammal. 2012;34(2):251–6. https://doi.org/10.1071/AM12012.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Halsey LG, Shepard ELC, Wilson RP. Assessing the development and application of the accelerometry technique for estimating energy expenditure. Comp Biochem Physiol A Mol Integr Physiol. 2011;158(3):305–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cbpa.2010.09.002.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Wilson RP, Börger L, Holton MD, Scantlebury DM, Gómez-Laich A, Quintana F, et al. Estimates for energy expenditure in free-living animals using acceleration proxies: a reappraisal. J Anim Ecol. 2020;89(1):161–72. https://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2656.13040.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Gleiss AC, Wilson RP, Shepard ELC. Making overall dynamic body acceleration work: On the theory of acceleration as a proxy for energy expenditure. Methods Ecol Evol. 2011;2(1):23–33. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2041-210X.2010.00057.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Ladds MA, Salton M, Hocking DP, Mcintosh RR, Thompson AP, Slip DJ, et al. Using accelerometers to develop time-energy budgets of wild fur seals from captive surrogates. PeerJ. 2018;6:e5814-e. https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.5814.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Speakman JR, Król E. Maximal heat dissipation capacity and hyperthermia risk: Neglected key factors in the ecology of endotherms. J Anim Ecol. 2010;79(4):726–46. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2656.2010.01689.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Fuller A, Hetem RS, Maloney SK, Mitchell D. Adaptation to heat and water shortage in large, arid-zone mammals. Physiology. 2014;29(3):159–67. https://doi.org/10.1152/physiol.00049.2013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Watterson I, Abbs D, Bhend J, Chiew F, Church J, Ekström M, et al. Rangelands cluster report, climate change in Australia projections for australia’s natural resource management regions: cluster reports. 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Bureau of Meteorology; 2017. Climate data online. Available from: http://www.bom.gov.au/climate/data/. Accessed 12 Oct 2017.

    Google Scholar 

  26. Tatler J, Prowse TA, Roshier DA, Cairns KM, Cassey P. Phenotypic variation and promiscuity in a wild population of pure dingoes (canis dingo). J Zool Syst Evol Res. 2021;59(1):311–22. https://doi.org/10.1111/jzs.12418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Tatler J, Prowse TA, Roshier DA, Allen BL, Cassey P. Resource pulses affect prey selection and reduce dietary diversity of dingoes in arid australia. Mammal Rev. 2019;49(3):263–75. https://doi.org/10.1111/mam.12157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Meek P, Jenkins D, Morris B, Ardler A, Hawksby R. Use of two humane leg-hold traps for catching pest species. Wildl Res. 1995;22(6):733–9. https://doi.org/10.1071/WR9950733.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2017.

    Google Scholar 

  30. Tatler J, Cassey P, Prowse TA. High accuracy at low frequency: Detailed behavioural classification from accelerometer data. J Exp Biol. 2018;221(23):jeb184085. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.184085.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Pagano AM, Williams TM. Estimating the energy expenditure of free-ranging polar bears using tri-axial accelerometers: A validation with doubly labeled water. Ecol Evol. 2019;9(7):4210–9. https://doi.org/10.1002/ece3.5053.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Shield J. Acclimation and energy metabolism of the dingo, cards dingo and the coyote, canis latrans. J Zool. 1972;168(4):483–501. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7998.1972.tb01363.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Shield J. Acclimation and energy metabolism of the dingo, canis dingo and the coyote, canis latrans. J Zool. 1972;168(4):483–501. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7998.1972.tb01363.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Bryce CM, Williams TM. Comparative locomotor costs of domestic dogs reveal energetic economy of wolf-like breeds. J Exp Biol. 2017;220(2):312–21. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.144188.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Mahoney SA. Cost of locomotion and heat balance during rest and running from 0 to 55 degrees c in a patas monkey. J Appl Physiol. 1980;49(5):789–800. https://doi.org/10.1152/jappl.1980.49.5.789.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Schmidt-Nielsen K. Animal physiology: adaptation and environment. 5th ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1997.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Department of Environment and Water; 2018. Naturemaps 3.0. Available from: https://data.environment.sa.gov.au/NatureMaps/Pages/default.aspx. Accessed 27 Mar 2018.

  38. Hijmans R, Van Etten J. Raster: Geographic analysis and modeling with raster data. R package version 2.6-7; 2010.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Dee DP, Uppala SM, Simmons AJ, Berrisford P, Poli P, Kobayashi S, et al. The era-interim reanalysis: Configuration and performance of the data assimilation system. Q J R Meteorol Soc. 2011;137(656):553–97. https://doi.org/10.1002/qj.828.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Didan K. In: DAAC NEL, editor. Mod13a2 modis/terra vegetation indices 16-day l3 global 1km sin grid v006 [modis land/terra vegetation indices 1-km 16-day (mod13a2 v6)]. 6th ed; 2015.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Bivand R, Lewin-Koh N. Maptools: Tools for handling spatial objects. R package version 0.9-4; 2018.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Tatler J. Integrated analysis of the movement and ecology of wild dingoes in the arid zone. Australia: University of Adelaide; 2019.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Bracis C, Bildstein KL, Mueller T. Revisitation analysis uncovers spatio-temporal patterns in animal movement data. Ecography. 2018;41(11):1801–11. https://doi.org/10.1111/ecog.03618.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. De'eath G. Mdm: Multinomial diversity model. R package version 1.3; 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Wood SN. Fast stable restricted maximum likelihood and marginal likelihood estimation of semiparametric generalized linear models. J R Stat Soc B (Statistical Methodology). 2011;73(1):3–36. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9868.2010.00749.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Bates D, Mächler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. 2015. 2015;67(1):48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01.

  47. Barton K. Mumin: Multi-model inference. 2009. http://r-forger-project.org/projects/mumin/.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Allen BL, Goullet M, Allen LR, Lisle A, Leung LKP. Dingoes at the doorstep: Preliminary data on the ecology of dingoes in urban areas. Landsc Urban Plan. 2013;119:131–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.landurbplan.2013.07.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Harmsen BJ, Foster RJ, Silver SC, Ostro LET, Doncaster CP. Jaguar and puma activity patterns in relation to their main prey. Mamm Biol. 2011;76(3):320–4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.mambio.2010.08.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Jenny D, Zuberbühler K. Hunting behaviour in west african forest leopards. Afr J Ecol. 2005;43(3):197–200. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2028.2005.00565.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Moseby KE, De Jong S, Munro N, Pieck A. Home range, activity and habitat use of european rabbits (oryctolagus cuniculus) in arid australia: Implications for control. Wildl Res. 2005;32(4):305–11. https://doi.org/10.1071/WR04013.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Aublet J-F, Festa-Bianchet M, Bergero D, Bassano B. Temperature constraints on foraging behaviour of male alpine ibex (capra ibex) in summer. Oecologia. 2009;159(1):237–47. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00442-008-1198-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Norris AL, Kunz TH. Effects of solar radiation on animal thermoregulation. In: Babatunde E, editor. Solar radiation. Croatia: IntechOpen; 2012. p. 195–220.

    Google Scholar 

  54. Cotton CL, Parker KL. Winter activity patterns of northern flying squirrels in sub-boreal forests. Can J Zool. 2000;78(11):1896–901. https://doi.org/10.1139/z00-137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Murray IW, Smith FA. Estimating the influence of the thermal environment on activity patterns of the desert woodrat (neotoma lepida) using temperature chronologies. Can J Zool. 2012;90(9):1171–80. https://doi.org/10.1139/z2012-084.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Mcnab BK. The physiological ecology of vertebrates: A view from energetics. Ithaca: Cornell University Press; 2002.

  57. Phillips CJ, Coppinger RP, Schimel DS. Hyperthermia in running sled dogs. J Appl Physiol. 1981;51(1):135–42. https://doi.org/10.1152/jappl.1981.51.1.135.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Pond CM. The significance of lactation in the evolution of mammals. Evolution. 1977;31(1):177–99. https://doi.org/10.2307/2407556.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Weber D. The ecological significance of resting sites and the seasonal habitat change in polecats (mustela putorius). J Zool. 1989;217(4):629–38. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7998.1989.tb02514.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Ruggiero LF, Pearson E, Henry SE. Characteristics of american marten den sites in Wyoming. J Wildl Manag. 1998;62(2):663–73. https://doi.org/10.2307/3802342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Baker PJ, Robertson CPJ, Funk SM, Harris S. Potential fitness benefits of group living in the red fox, vulpes vulpes. Anim Behav. 1998;56(6):1411–24. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1998.0950.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Hetem RS, Mitchell D, De Witt BA, Fick LG, Maloney SK, Meyer LCR, et al. Body temperature, activity patterns and hunting in free-living cheetah: biologging reveals new insights. Integr Zool. 2019;14(1):30–47. https://doi.org/10.1111/1749-4877.12341.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Rabaiotti D, Woodroffe R. Coping with climate change: Limited behavioral responses to hot weather in a tropical carnivore. Oecologia. 2019;189(3):587–99. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00442-018-04329-1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Murray Schofield, Mark and Tess McLaren, Keith Bellchambers, Hannah Bannister, Casey O’Brien and all other volunteers for their role in data collection at Kalamurina. We also acknowledge the constructive comments given by John Kanowski on the manuscript. This project was undertaken in collaboration with the Australian Wildlife Conservancy. JT acknowledges the support received through the provision of an Australian Government Research Training Program Scholarship. Funding was provided by the Australian Wildlife Conservancy, Nature Foundation of South Australia and Sir Mark Mitchell Research Fund.

Funding

JT was funded by a Faculty of Sciences, University of Adelaide postgraduate scholarship, and also by an Australian Government Research Training Scholarship. The project was funded by the Linnean Society of New South Wales, the Australian Wildlife Society, the Mark Mitchell Research Foundation and the Nature Conservancy of South Australia.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

JT, SEC and PC conceived the ideas and designed methodology. JT and DR collected the data. JT, AS and TP analysed the data. JT and SEC led the writing of the manuscript. All authors contributed critically to the drafts. The author(s) read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jack Tatler.

Ethics declarations

Consent for publication

All authors gave final approval for publication.

Competing interests

No competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1: Table S1.

Proportion of each day spent stationary, walking, and running. Table S2. Model summary showing the effect of landscape features, time of day, and period on dingo activity (ODBA). Model estimates, standard errors (SE) and p-values for our correlated intercepts and slopes linear mixed model are presented. Significance is indicated in bold

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Tatler, J., Currie, S.E., Cassey, P. et al. Accelerometer informed time-energy budgets reveal the importance of temperature to the activity of a wild, arid zone canid. Mov Ecol 9, 11 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40462-021-00246-w

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40462-021-00246-w

Keywords