Skip to main content

Effects of habitat modifications on the movement behavior of animals: the case study of Fish Aggregating Devices (FADs) and tropical tunas

Abstract

Background

Aggregation sites represent important sources of environmental heterogeneity and can modify the movement behavior of animals. When these sites are artificially established through anthropogenic actions, the consequent alterations to animal movements may impact their ecology with potential implications for their fitness. Floating objects represent important sources of habitat heterogeneity for tropical tunas, beneath which these species naturally aggregate in large numbers. Man-made floating objects, called Fish Aggregating Devices (FAD), are used by fishers on a massive scale to facilitate fishing operations. In addition to the direct impacts that fishing with FADs has on tuna populations, assessing the effects of increasing the numbers of FADs on the ecology of tuna is key for generating sound management and conservation measures.

Methods

This study investigates the effects of increasing numbers of FADs (aggregation sites) on the movements of tunas, through the comparison of electronic tagging data recorded from 146 individuals tunas (yellowfin tuna, Thunnus albacares, and skipjack tuna, Katsuwonus pelamis) tagged in three instrumented anchored FAD arrays (Mauritius, Oahu-Hawaii and Maldives), that differed according to their distances among neighboring FADs. The effect of increasing inter-FAD distances is studied considering a set of indices (residence times at FADs and absence (travel) times between two visits at FADs) and their trends.

Results

When inter-FAD distances decrease, tuna visit more FADs (higher connectivity between FADs), spend less time travelling between FADs and more time associated with them. The trends observed for the absence (travel) times appear to be compatible with a random-search component in the movement behaviour of tunas. Conversely, FAD residence times showed opposite trends, which could be a result of social behavior and/or prey availability.

Conclusion

Our results provide the first evidence of changes in tuna associative behavior for increasing FAD densities. More generally, they highlight the need for comparing animal movements in heterogeneous habitats in order to improve understanding of the impacts of anthropogenic habitat modifications on the ecology of wild animals.

Introduction

Animals evolve in a heterogeneous environment with which they interact through physiological and/or behavioral responses. As a result of habitat fragmentation, barrier effects, or resource changes, animal movements can change at multiple scales [35] and their migration routes can be altered [47]. Anthropogenic activities leading to modification in habitats can also result in changes in the way animals use and move through their environment. Currently, little is known about the behavioral plasticity of animals when environmental conditions change [45]. In the terrestrial habitat, waterholes represent important sources of habitat heterogeneity for several species [36, 48], as do floating objects for several fish species, in the pelagic habitat of the world’s oceans. Indeed, many pelagic fish, including tropical tunas (yellowfin tuna, Thunnus albacares, bigeye tuna, T. obesus and skipjack tuna, Katsuwonus pelamis), form large multi-specific aggregations around floating objects [6, 17]. Naturally occurring, floating objects include debris from trees and logs, which enter the ocean via river months or from the coast. Our understanding of the reasons that lead to such aggregations remains limited [6, 17]. Two main hypotheses have been formulated: the “meeting point” and the “indicator-log”. The “meeting point” hypothesis suggests that floating objects can be meeting points for tunas, facilitating the formation of larger schools [10, 17], which provide benefits to these social species. The “indicator-log” hypothesis asserts that tunas use floating objects as indicators of rich environments, as natural floating objects typically originate from highly productive areas (i.e., river mouth, mangroves) [21]. For centuries, fishers have used this associative behavior to their advantages [14], first targeting natural floating objects and then deploying man-made floating devices called Fish Aggregating Devices (FADs) in order to attract tunas and enhance the efficiency of their fishing effort [16, 34]. About, 40% of the world’s tropical tuna catch consists of fish associated with floating objects [8]. Coastal anchored FADs are generally deployed near shore to support artisanal tuna fisheries while drifting FADs are utilized offshore by industrial purse seine fisheries. The increasing use of FADs since the 1990’s has resulted in thousands of artificial floating objects being deployed in the open ocean on a regular basis, changing the pelagic habitat [1, 8, 9, 32]. Such modification has led to concerns regarding the impacts of FADs on the ecology of tunas [8, 9, 22, 31], as FADs could act as ecological traps (see [2, 44]). These studies highlighted the need for an improved understanding of the effects of increasing numbers of FADs on the behavior of tunas. The model developed by Sempo et al. [46] demonstrated that, for social fish species, increasing the number of floating objects could lead to fragmentation of fish schools. However, an evaluation of the effects of increasing numbers of FADs on tuna behaviour, through field-based experiments, is yet to be conducted. Both anchored and drifting FADs alter the natural environment, but anchored FADs are easier to access and study owing to their close proximity to the coast and fixed location. As such, understanding tuna behavior around anchored FADs can improve the general understanding of tunas behavior in relation to drifting floating objects [11]. Many studies have investigated the behavior of tuna at anchored FADs, using active tracking [4, 7, 23, 30, 43]⁠ or passive acoustic telemetry [12, 19, 37, 41, 42]⁠. FAD associations were observed to last from a few minutes to several weeks [12, 19, 37, 41, 42], clearly illustrating that FADs are able to retain tuna. Fitting survival curves to acoustic telemetry data showed that the behavioral process driving the time that tuna spend at FADs is independent of time [41, 42]. Using passive acoustic telemetry in the Oahu FAD array, Dagorn et al. [12] found that 17% of the observed travel times of tagged tuna between two FAD associations were short enough to be coherent with straight line movements. This was in line with results from several active tracking studies which observed tunas making “straight-line” movements between FADs [4, 7, 13, 23, 30]. Analysing these movements, Girard et al. [18] demonstrated that tunas are capable of oriented movements towards FADs from 4 to 17 km away, while for larger distances, tuna movements seemed to be driven by random-walk dynamics. From these studies, it is possible to hypothesize that tuna movements in a FAD array could be broken down into two components (or processes): (i) tuna move randomly as long as they do not perceive the presence of a FAD and (ii) they orient towards a FAD when they detect its presence, which could occur from 4 to 17 km from the FAD, follow the results of Girard et al. [18]. Finally, none of these studies have focused on the role of FAD densities on tuna behavior for a particular species and size class.

The main objective of our study was to investigate the effects of increasing the number of aggregation sites on the movement behavior of animals. For this purpose we used the FAD associative behavior of tunas as a case study, as this is currently a major conservation concern for fisheries management bodies ([25]; ISSF: [40])⁠. Our methodological approach was to compare the behavior of tuna (characterized through the detection of tagged individuals by receivers attached to FADs) among three different instrumented anchored FAD arrays in Mauritius and the Maldives in the Indian Ocean and Oahu-Hawaii in the Pacific Ocean, each with different distances between neighboring FADs (i.e. different FAD densities).

Materials & methods

FAD arrays

We used data collected from studies conducted around three anchored FAD arrays located around (i) the island of Mauritius [42] in the Western Indian Ocean (ii) the island of Oahu [41], within the Hawaiian archipelago, in the Central Pacific Ocean and (iii) the Maldivian archipelago [19] in the central Indian Ocean (Fig. 1). All FADs were moored in water between 1000 and 2500 m deep. FADs from the same array all had the same design, but designs differed slightly between arrays. The design of FADs, however, has never been identified to have an effect on the attractiveness of the floating objects, as tunas have been found around all types of floating object (see [17]). The Mauritian FAD array was composed of 9 FADs located on the western side of the island [42]. The Hawaiian array consisted of 13 FADs moored around the island of Oahu [41]. In the Maldives the array consisted of 54 FADs moored around the entire archipelago [19] (Table 1). The protocol for the three field studies was to exhaustively instrument all FADs of the same array (Hawaii) or sub-array (Mauritius, Maldives) (Fig. 1), in order to observe all movements between FADs located in the same area.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Location of the three anchored FAD arrays. The positions of anchored FADs are represented by a black dot when they were equipped with an acoustic receiver, and by a black cross when they did not have acoustic receivers

Table 1 Characteristics of the three instrumented FAD arrays

These three anchored FAD arrays were chosen as they each have different FAD densities, i.e., they offer different inter-FAD distances. Both the nearest-neighbor and next-nearest neighbor distances (Fig. 2a and b) were significantly different between the three instrumented FAD arrays (Dunn post hoc test, p-value< 0.05), see Fig. 2, and followed an increasing gradient from Mauritius – Hawaii – Maldives.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Inter-FAD distances of each study area. a nearest neighbor inter-FAD distances. b next-nearest neighbor inter-FAD distances. Dunn post hoc test with p-values adjusted by the Holm method: *** indicates p < 0.001, ** p < 0.01, and * p < 0.05

Acoustic tagging database

During dedicated acoustic telemetry experiments, the three anchored FAD arrays were instrumented with acoustic receivers which detect the presence of tagged fish within a given detection range. Details of the telemetry experiments are described in Govinden et al. [19], Robert et al. [41] and Rodriguez-Tress et al. [42]. The available data sets collected during these experiments consisted of acoustic detections from 49, 92 and 52 tropical tunas tagged in the FAD arrays of Mauritius, Hawaii and the Maldives, respectively. The movements of tunas within the instrumented FAD arrays were monitored for between 38 and 50 days in Mauritius, from 3 months to 1 year in the Maldives and for more than 1 year in Hawaii.

The available detection data were grouped into cohorts according to species, size category and FAD array (Additional file 1). Additional file 2 provides the detailed numbers of tagged tuna within each category. To allow for the comparison of tuna behavior between different FAD arrays, only cohorts present in at least two FAD arrays, with at least 9 tagged individuals, were selected for the analysis (Additional file 2). This resulted in three species-size categories: yellowfin tuna of ~ 70 cm (YFT-70; fork length ranges: 60–80 cm), yellowfin tuna of ~ 50 cm (YFT-50; fork length ranges: 40–60 cm) and skipjack of ~ 50 cm (SKJ-50; fork length ranges: 40–60 cm). In total 146 tagged individuals were retained for the analysis. The YFT-70 category allowed for comparison between the Mauritian and Hawaiian FAD arrays with 14 and 56 individuals, respectively. The SKJ-50 category allowed for comparison between the Mauritian and the Maldivian FAD arrays with 15 and 22 individuals, respectively. The YFT-50 category was the only one where the comparison among the three FAD arrays was possible with 11, 9 and 19 tuna from Mauritius, Hawaii and the Maldives, respectively (Table 2).

Table 2 Indices for each species-size category and FAD array

Data analysis: associative and movement behavior

For each category, the time that tuna spent at (residence time), or away from (absence time) FADs was calculated. The residence times corresponded to the Continuous Residence Times (CRT) [5, 12, 37], which reflected the duration within which a tagged fish was continuously detected at the same FAD without day-scale (> 24 h) absences and without being detected at any other FAD in the array. The time that tuna spent away from FADs was defined as the Continuous Absence Times (CAT) [5, 19]. A CAT corresponded to the time interval between two consecutive CRTs. The times spent away from FADs were separated into two categories: (i) absences with returns to the same FAD of origin following periods longer than 24 h or, (ii) movements between different FADs in the array. Accordingly, the nature of consecutive CRTs (two consecutive visits to the same FAD or at different FADs) resulted in two different types of CATs: CATreturn(s) and CATdiff(s).

The durations of CRT, CATreturn and CATdiff, were compared between FAD arrays for each species-size category, using the data recorded during the full study period for each array. When only two arrays were compared, a Mann-Whitney test [29] was performed. Where the three AFAD arrays were compared, a pairwise approach was used and a Kruskal-Wallis test [28] applied, followed by a Dunn post hoc test [15] with a p-value adjusted by the Holm method [24].

Finally, the number of return movements (NCATreturn) and the number of movements between different FADs (NCATdiff), as well as the total number of residences times (NCRT) were also compared between FAD arrays for each species-size category. The total number of movements and residence times recorded over the full monitoring period within each array is shown in Additional file 3. To allow for comparison between study areas, each with different monitoring durations, only the number of residence times and movements recorded during the first 38 days of observation for all tuna tagged in all three FAD arrays were considered (Additional file 3). This duration is the shortest observation time recorded for a tuna in the three FAD arrays (it was recorded for a 50-cm YFT in the Mauritian array). Furthermore, NCATreturn, NCATdiff and NCRT were divided by the number of tuna tagged for each FAD array, to account for differences in tagging effort (Table 2 and Additonal file 5). The trends of these indices (CRT, CATreturn, CATdiff, NCRT, NCATreturn and NCATdiff) were then compared with the output of a simple random-walk simulation, without any orientation nor retention component, for which all details are provided in Additional file 4.

All analyses were conducted using the R software (R Core Team 2018 version 3.4.4). Mann-Whitney tests were performed with the function “wilcox.test” (two-sample) in the “stat” package, Kruskal-Wallis tests were performed with the function “kruskal.test” in the “stat” package and Dunn tests were performed using the function “posthoc.kruskal.dunn.test” in the “PMCMR” package [39].

Results

Residence times

For the two YFT species-size categories, CRTs decreased significantly with increasing inter-FAD distances (Mann-Whitney test: p-value< 0.05 for YFT-70 and Dunn post hoc test p-value< 0.05 for YFT-50, Fig. 3a and b) and differences for SKJ were marginally significant (Mann-Whitney test: p-value = 0.067, Fig. 3c). In other words, the closer the FADs, the more time tuna spent in continuous association with the same FAD without interruption from visits to other FADs, or long excursions (> 24 h). The same significant decrease was observed for the number of CRTs for the three species-size categories (Mann-Whitney test: p-value< 0.05, Additional file 5), except for YFT-50 between Mauritius and Hawaii (Dunn post hoc test: p-value = 0.829, Additional file 5F). The average duration and number of CRTs per tagged individual for each species-size category and FAD array is shown in Table 2.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Duration of the CRT, CATdiff(s) and CATreturn(s) for the experimental data for each AFAD array and specie-size category. YFT-70 (a, d and g), YFT-50 (b, e and h) and SKJ-50 (c, f and i). Mann-Whitney test, except for YFT-50 where a Dunn post hoc test with p-values adjusted by the Holm method was performed: *** indicates p < 0.001, ** p < 0.01, and * p < 0.05

Absence times

The mean numbers and durations of CATdiff and CATreturn recorded for each species-size category and FAD array are shown in Table 2. For YFT-70, the durations of CATdiff and CATreturn showed a significant increasing trend between Mauritius and Hawaii (Mann-Whitney: p-value< 0.05, Fig. 3d and g). This finding suggests that the greater the inter-FAD distances, the more time tuna spend away from FADs. Remarkably, no CAT (neither CATdiff nor CATreturn) was recorded in the Maldivian array (Table 2 and Fig. 3), which had the largest inter-FAD distances. This absence of CATs was significantly different from the number of CATs recorded in the other FAD arrays (see Additional file 5).

Discussion

Habitat heterogeneity can play a crucial role in the movement ecology of marine animals. Relatively small structures can significantly impact animal movements either by inducing an attractive or retentive effect, or a combination of the two, resulting in animals spending a disproportionate amount of time in their proximity. Modifying the abundance of such structures can alter the movements of an animal, but very few studies have investigated the mechanisms by which such changes could result in alterations to animal behaviour [3, 27, 38]. Floating objects represent a key source of habitat heterogeneity for tropical tunas and other pelagic species, although the exact role played by these objects in the ontogeny or ecology of many species remains uncertain. While objects floating on the ocean’s surface were historically only of natural origins (e.g. logs), the number of artificial floating objects (e.g., FADs) has increased considerably in recent decades, surpassing the number of natural floating objects in some areas [9]. This addition of artificial objects to the pelagic environment is due to the significant fishing advantages that these devices afford. While concerns were raised more than two decades ago over the potential impacts these artificial objects could have on the ecology of tunas [31], no study has yet investigated, at a fine-scale, the effects of increasing the distances between floating objects on the movements of tunas. For the first time, the current study undertook a comparative analysis of the associative behavior of tunas with FADs, across multiple anchored FAD arrays with differing inter-FAD distances, in order to investigate how this factor could influence tuna movements.

Absence time

The observed trends of CATdiff and CATreturn, which increased with inter-FAD distances, were similar to those found for the random walker (Fig. 4 and see Additional file 4). Knowing that CATdiff(s) and CATreturn(s) correspond to periods when tagged fish are undetected, the significant absence of CATdiff in the Maldives for all species and size categories may be interpreted as tuna performing excursions away from FADs longer than the observation time, as was observed in the random-walk simulations (see Additional file 4). The absence of CATreturn(s) in the Maldivian FAD array may also be compatible with the random-walk trends observed in the simulations for finite observation times. Indeed, the simulated total number of CATreturn(s) is expected to decrease at large inter-FAD distances and is likely to be very small for finite observation times (less than one CATreturn per simulated individual for inter-FAD distances larger than 30 km, see Additional file 4, Figure S2).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Summary of trends of three indices of movement behaviour. Trends are shown when inter-FAD distances increases. The first column shows the results of the random-walk simulations; the third column shows the results from the telemetry data analysis (MAU.: Mauritius, OAH.: Oahu, MAL.: Maldives)

Despite the observed trends being compatible with those found for the simple random-walk model, the increased duration of movement between two different FADs (CATdiff) when inter-FAD distances increase may also be explained by tunas swimming in a straight line (oriented movement) from one FAD to another. With such oriented movements, the duration is proportional to the distance, and shorter than for movements with random components. Indeed, for a 50-cm individual with a speed of one body length per second, these durations would be 3.3 h, 8 h and 21 h for the Mauritian, Hawaiian and Maldivian array, respectively (mean neighboring inter-FAD distance of 6, 15 and 38 km, respectively, see Table 1). However, our analysis shows (Table 2) that durations of inter-FAD movements for 50-cm yellowfin tuna correspond in average to 0.6 (±0.11) days (±) and 11 (±15) days in the Mauritian and Hawaiian FAD arrays, respectively, while no CATdiff were recorded in the Maldivian array. These values are larger than those expected for straight line movements between FADs. Previous studies of Girard et al. [18] found that tuna movements between FADs consist of two components: (i) oriented movements towards FADs at short distances and (ii) a random-walk dynamics at large distances. Adding an additional directed movement to FADs within a given radius to the random-walk behaviour will not change the trends in CATdiff and CATreturn found for the simple random walk model. However, fitting a model that accounts for both this directed movement component and for the actual geometries of the FAD arrays (including the presence of the islands) on the field data is key for testing the hypothesis of a random-walk component in tuna movements more quantitatively.

Residence time

Conversely, our results show that the evolution of CRT with increasing inter-FAD distances does not follow the trends found for the simple random-walk model (Fig. 4). This result outlines that the observed trends cannot be ascribed to the modified array of acoustic receivers but are a true signal of a change in tuna behavior (see Additional file 4). While possible effects of FAD densities in line with those predicted by the random-walk model cannot be excluded, the observed trends in CRT for increasing FAD distances indicate that the association time is driven by other processes. In this respect, the ‘meeting point’ [17] may explain a significant decrease of CRT for increasing inter-FAD distances. This hypothesis states that tunas could use FADs as meeting points to form larger schools. In simple terms, tunas could remain associated with FADs until the aggregation is large enough, then leave the FAD. When FADs are very numerous, under certain circumstances the local tuna population could be evenly distributed among FADs [46]. In such cases, the higher the FAD density, the smaller the sizes of the tuna aggregations under each FAD, and the longer they would stay at FADs in order to wait for their school to be large enough to leave (according to the ‘meeting point’). This effect may be amplified for increasing sizes of the tuna population in an area. As such, a large tuna population would imply larger aggregation sizes and thus shorter residence times. Considering that the Maldives has one of the largest anchored FAD fisheries in the world [19], significantly greater than in Mauritius and Hawaii, their (transient) local tuna population is certainly larger than those in the two other arrays. Consequently, under the meeting point hypothesis, the shorter recorded CRTs in the Maldivian array could be explained by the fastest formation of large tuna aggregations around FADs, due to both the low FAD density and the higher density of tuna within the array. This interpretation is valid when comparing the Mauritian and Maldivian arrays, as well as the Hawaiian and Maldivian arrays, but there is insufficient evidence to conclude that there is a difference in tuna density between the Mauritian and the Hawaiian arrays.

Ultimately, the trends in CRTs may also be explained by the availability of prey in the three anchored FAD arrays, as proposed in previous studies [37, 41]. Indeed, several studies demonstrated that tuna feed primarily on species that do not associate with FADs [20, 26, 33]. Since FADs do not have an important trophic function for tuna, individuals likely feed during their excursions away of FADs. The local availability of prey within the anchored FAD arrays may therefore affect the amount of time tuna spend at FADs. Consequently, longer residence times at FADs could correspond to higher prey abundance (and/or higher accessibility) in the Mauritian anchored FAD array than in the Hawaiian and Maldivian arrays.

Conclusion and perspectives

The trends observed from tagging data for the absence times (travel time between two associations) appear to be consistent with the existence of a random search component in the behavior of tuna within a FAD array, in addition to the oriented component of the movements observed when tunas are a few kilometres from the FADs [18]. Conversely, the observed trends in FAD residence times appear to be driven by more complex processes involving, for instance, inter-individual interactions at the FADs, such as those suggested by the meeting point hypothesis, or due to prey availability. As our study is the first to focus on the role of FAD densities (inter-FAD distances) on tuna behavior for a particular species and size class, further studies on different FAD arrays would be necessary to strengthen our conclusions. However, we find the same trends for the three specie-size categories for the three arrays, knowing that one category compares the three arrays (YFT-50: Mauritius, Hawaii and the Maldives), and the two other compare two different arrays (YFT-70: Mauritius and Hawaii; SKJ-50: Mauritius and the Maldives).

Considering that when the FAD density increases, the connectivity between FADs increases (more FADs are visited), tuna spend shorter times un-associated and exhibit longer residence times, then the total time tunas spend in the FAD array will likely increase. Dedicated research is needed to quantify such change, for instance through a model of the behavior of tuna combining a random walk component with an oriented component.

The increase of the number of floating objects in the ocean would lead to increases in the time tuna spend at FADs, which would increase their vulnerability to fisheries. Such consequence can be extended to all species that associate with floating objects, including bycatch species such as dolphinfish (Coryphaena hippurus) or silky sharks (Carcharhinus falciformis), a vulnerable species. Consequently, understanding the effects of increasing the density of FADs on tuna behavior is clearly a major research objective for generating sound management and conservation measures to ensure the sustainability of this fishing practice.

In a broader context, this study can be extended to all wild animals that present an associative behavior with specific structures. We can cite for instance the importance of waterholes for terrestrial wild animals. The number and location of waterholes impact the space use by wild animals and their movement, as well as the viability of their populations, as for example if waterholes are inside or outside of protected areas [36, 48]. Changes of the number and/or location of waterholes may be caused by drought due to climate change [36] or caused by agriculture (livestock) [48]. In this respect, this study could open novel perspectives for comparing animal movements in a changing environment and improve knowledge on the impacts of global change on their ecology.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on request.

References

  1. Baske A, Gibbon J, Benn J, Nickson A. Estimating the use of drifting Fish Aggregation Devices (FADs ) around the globe, Pew discussion paper; 2012. p. 8.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Battin J. When goods animals love bad habitats: ecological trap and the conservation of animal populations. Conserv Biol. 2004;18(6):1482–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Boutin S. Food supplementation experiments with terrestrial vertebrates: patterns, problems, and the future. Can J Zool. 1990;68(2):203–20. https://doi.org/10.1139/z90-031.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Brill RW, Block BA, Boggs CH, Bigelow KA, Freund EV, Marcinek DJ. Horizontal movements and depth distribution of large adult yellowfin tuna (Thunnus albacares) near the Hawaiian islands, recorded using ultrasonic telemetry: implications for hte physiological ecology of pelagic fishes. Mar Biol. 1999;133:395–408. https://doi.org/10.1007/s002270050478.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Capello M, Robert M, Soria M, Potin G, Itano D, Holland K, Deneubourg J-L, Dagorn L. A methodological framework to estimate the site fidelity of tagged animals using passive acoustic telemetry. PLoS One. 2015;10(8):1–19. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0134002.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Castro J, Santiago J, Santana-Ortega T. A general theory on fish aggregation to floating objects: an alternative to the meeting point hypothesis. Rev Fish Biol Fish. 2002;11:255–77. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1020302414472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Cayré P. Behaviour of yellowfin tuna (Thunnus albacares) and skipjack tuna (Katsuwonus pelamis) around fish aggregating devices (FADs) in the Comoros islands as determinated by ultrasonie tagging. Aquat Living Resour. 1991;4:1–12. https://doi.org/10.1051/alr/1991000.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Dagorn L, Holland KN, Restrepo V, Moreno G. Is it good or bad to fish with FADs? What are the real impacts of the use of drifting FADs on pelagic marine ecosystems? Fish Fish. 2013a;14(3):391–415. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-2979.2012.00478.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Dagorn L, Bez N, Fauvel T, Walker E. How much do fish aggregating devices (FADs) modify the floating object environment in the ocean? Fish Oceanogr. 2013b;22(3):147–53. https://doi.org/10.1111/fog.12014.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Dagorn L, Fréon P. Tropical tuna associated with floating objects: a simulation study of the meeting point hypothesis. Can J Fish Aquat Sci. 1999;56(6):984–93. https://doi.org/10.1139/cjfas-56-6-984.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Dagorn L, Holland KN, Filmalter JD. Are drifting FADs essential for testing the ecological trap hypothesis? Fish Res. 2010;106(1):60–3. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.fishres.2010.07.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Dagorn L, Holland KN, Itano D. Behavior of yellowfin (Thunnus albacares) and bigeye (T. obesus) tuna in a network of fish aggregating devices (FADs). Mar Biol. 2007;151(2):595–606. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00227-006-0511-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Dagorn L, Josse E, Bach P, Bertrand A. Modeling tuna behaviour near floating objects: from individuals to aggregations. Aquat Living Resour. 2000;13(4):203–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0990-7440(00)01065-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Dempster T, Taquet M. Fish aggregation device (FAD) research: gaps in current knowledge and future directions for ecological studies. Rev Fish Biol Fish. 2004;14(1):21–42. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11160-004-3151-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Dunn OJ. Multiple comparisons among means. J Am Stat Assoc. 1961;56(293):52–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Fonteneau A, Pallarés P, Pianet R. A worldwide review of purse seine fisheries on FADs. In: Pêche Thonière et Dispositifs de Concentration de Poissons, Caribbean-Martinique, 15-19 Oct 1999; 2000. p. 15–35.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Fréon P, Dagorn L. Review of fish associative behaviour: toward a generalisation of the meeting point hypothesis. Rev Fish Biol Fish. 2000;10(2):183–207. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1016666108540.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Girard C, Benhamou S, Dagorn L. FAD: fish aggregating device or fish attracting device? A new analysis of yellowfin tuna movements around floating objects. Anim Behav. 2004;67(2):319–26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.07.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Govinden R, Jauhary R, Filmalter JD, Forget F, Soria M, Adam S, Dagorn L. Movement behaviour of skipjack (Katsuwonus pelamis) and yellowfin (Thunnus albacares) tuna at anchored fish aggregating devices (FADs) in the Maldives, investigated by acoustic telemetry. Aquat Living Resour. 2013;26(1):69–77. https://doi.org/10.1051/alr/2012022.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Graham BS, Grubbs D, Holland K, Popp BN. A rapid ontogenetic shift in the diet of juvenile yellowfin tuna from Hawaii. Mar Biol. 2007;150(4):647–58. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00227-006-0360-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Hall MA. Some hypotheses on the mechanisms governing the association of tunas with floating objects and dolphins, the association of tunas with floating objects and dolphins in the eastern Pacific Ocean. In: International workshop on the ecology and fisheries for tunas associated with floating objects and on assessment issues arising from the association of tunas with floating objects; 1992. (January).

    Google Scholar 

  22. Hallier JP, Gaertner D. Drifting fish aggregation devices could act as an ecological trap for tropical tuna species. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2008;353:255–64. https://doi.org/10.3354/meps07180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Holland KN, Brill RW, Chang RKC. Horizontal and vertical movements of yellowfin and bigeye tuna associated with fish aggregating devices. Fish Bull. 1990;88(3):493–507.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Holm S. A simple sequentially Rejective multiple test procedure. Scand J Stat. 1979;6(2):65–70.

    Google Scholar 

  25. IOTC. Report of the 22nd session of the IOTC scientific committee: IOTC; 2019. p. 204. Retrieved from http://www.iotc.org/meetings/search?s=&field_meeting_tid_i18n=68&field_meeting_year_tid=All.

  26. Jaquemet S, Potier M, Ménard F. Do drifting and anchored fish aggregating devices (FADs) similarly influence tuna feeding habits? A case study from the western Indian Ocean. Fish Res. 2011;107(1–3):283–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.fishres.2010.11.011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Knight J. Making wildlife viewable: habituation and attraction. Soc Anim. 2009;17(2):167–84. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853009X418091.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Kruskal WH, Wallis WA. Use of ranks in one-criterion variance analysis. J Am Stat Assoc. 1952;47(260):583–621.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Mann HB, Whitney DR. On a test of whether one of two random variables is stochastically larger than the other. Ann Math Stat. 1947;18(1):50–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Marsac F, Cayré P. Telemetry applied to behaviour analysis of yellowfin tuna (Thunnus albacares, Bonnaterre, 1788) movements in a network of fish aggregating devices. Hydrobiologia. 1998;371(372):155–71. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1017009624211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Marsac F, Fonteneau A, Ménard F. Drifting FADs used in tuna fisheries: an ecological trap? In: Biology and behaviour of pelagic fish aggregations; 2000. p. 537–52.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Maufroy A, Kaplan DM, Bez N, De Molina AD, Murua H, Floch L, Chassot E. Massive increase in the use of drifting Fish Aggregating Devices (dFADs) by tropical tuna purse seine fisheries in the Atlantic and Indian oceans. ICES J Mar Sci. 2016;74(1):215–25. https://doi.org/10.1093/icesjms/fsw175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Ménard F, Fonteneau A, Gaertner D, Nordstrom V, Stéquert B, Marchal E. Exploitation of small tunas by a purse-seine fishery with fish aggregating devices and their feeding ecology in an eastern tropical Atlantic ecosystem. ICES J Mar Sci. 2000;57(3):525–30. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmsc.2000.0717.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Naeem A. Fish Aggregating Devices (FADs) in the Maldives. Rasain Annu J Maldivian Minist Fish Agric. 1988;8:179–200.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Nams VO. Using animal movement paths to measure response to spatial scale. Oecologia. 2005;143(2):179–88. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00442-004-1804-z.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. O’Farrill G, Schampaert KG, Rayfield B, Bodin Ö, Calmé S, Sengupta R, Gonzalez A. The potential connectivity of waterhole networks and the effectiveness of a protected area under various drought scenarios. PLoS One. 2014;9(5). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0095049.

  37. Ohta I, Kakuma S. Periodic behavior and residence time of yellowfin and bigeye tuna associated with fish aggregating devices around Okinawa Islands, as identified with automated listening stations. Mar Biol. 2005;146(3):581–94. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00227-004-1456-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Orams MB. Feeding wildlife as a tourism attraction: a review of issues and impacts. Tour Manag. 2002;23(3):281–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0261-5177(01)00080-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Pohlert T. The Pairwise Multiple Comparison of Mean Ranks Package (PMCMR). R package; 2014.

  40. Restrepo, V., Koehler, H., Moreno, G., & Murua, H. (2019). Recommended best pratices for FAD managment in tropical tun purse seine fisheries. ISSF technical report.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Robert M, Dagorn L, Filmalter JD, Deneubourg J-L, Itano D, Holland K. Intra-individual behavioral variability displayed by tuna at fish aggregating devices (FADs). Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2013;484:239–47. https://doi.org/10.3354/meps10303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Rodriguez-Tress P, Capello M, Forget F, Soria M, Beeharry SP, Dussooa N, Dagorn L. Associative behavior of yellowfin Thunnus albacares, skipjack Katsuwonus pelamis, and bigeye tuna T. obesus at anchored fish aggregating devices (FADs) off the coast of Mauritius. Mar Ecol Prog Ser. 2017;570:213–22. https://doi.org/10.3354/meps12101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Schaefer KM, Fuller DW. Behavior of bigeye (Thunnus obesus) and skipjack (Katsuwonus pelamis) tunas within aggregations associated with floating objects in the equatorial eastern Pacific. Mar Biol. 2005;146(4):781–92. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00227-004-1480-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Schlaepfer MA, Runge MC, Sherman PW. Ecological and evolutionary traps. Trends Ecol Evol. 2002;17(10):474–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0169-5347(02)02580-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Sears MW, Riddell EA, Rusch TW, Angilletta MJ. The world still is not flat: lessons learned from organismal interactions with environmental heterogeneity in terrestrial environments. Integr Comp Biol. 2019;59(4):1049–58. https://doi.org/10.1093/icb/icz130.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Sempo G, Dagorn L, Robert M, Deneubourg J-L. Impact of increasing deployment of artificial floating objects on the spatial distribution of social fish species. J Appl Ecol. 2013;50(5):1081–92. https://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2664.12140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Tucker MA, Böhning-gaese K, Fagan WF, Fryxell JM, Van Moorter B, Alberts SC, et al. Moving in the Anthropocene: global reductions in terrestrial mammalian movements. Science. 2018;469(January):466–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Zvidzai M, Murwira A, Caron A, de Garine-Wichatitksy M. Waterhole use patterns at the wildlife/livestock interface in a semi-arid savanna of southern Africa. Int J Dev Sustain. 2013;2(2):455–71.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

 The authors would like to acknowledge the two anonymous reviewers for their extensive and constructive feedback.

Funding

This project was co-funded by the ANR project BLUEMED (ANR-14-ACHN-0002), “Observatoire des Ecosystèmes Pélagiques Tropicaux exploités” (Ob7) from IRD/MARBEC and by the International Pole-and-line Foundation (IPNLF).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

FF, JDF, SB, KH, DI, SA and RJ collected the raw data in the field. GP analysed the data and wrote the paper with major contribution of MC, LD, JLD and JDF. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Géraldine Pérez.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1.

Number of tuna tagged per tuna size for each FAD arrays and tuna species.

Additional file 2.

Number of tuna tagged in each FAD array per species and size category.

Additional file 3.

Number of tuna, number of CRTs, CATreturn(s) and CATdiff(s) for the full observation time and for only the first 38 days of observation.

Additional file 4.

Random-walk model.

Additional file 5.

Number of CRT (NCRT), CATdiff (NCATdiff) and CATreturn (NCATreturn) per individuals for each AFAD array and each specie-size category. YFT-70 (A, D and G), YFT-50 (B, E and H) and SKJ-50 (C, F and I). Mann-Whitney test, except for YFT-50 where a Dunn post hoc test with p-values adjusted by the Holm method was perfornmed: *** indicates p < 0.001, ** p < 0.01, and * p < 0.05. Binomial test: ### indicates p < 0.001, ## p < 0.01, and # p < 0.05.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Pérez, G., Dagorn, L., Deneubourg, JL. et al. Effects of habitat modifications on the movement behavior of animals: the case study of Fish Aggregating Devices (FADs) and tropical tunas. Mov Ecol 8, 47 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40462-020-00230-w

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40462-020-00230-w

Keywords