Skip to main content

Flexible resource use strategies of a central-place forager experiencing dynamic risk and opportunity

Abstract

Background

Movement decisions made in space and time define how wildlife meet competing extrinsic and intrinsic demands to maximize fitness. Differential selection of resource patches provides one example of how to measure how animals balance conflicting demands. We hypothesized that individual spatial selection of patch types between dynamic seasons would signify flexible strategies used to minimize risk and optimize foraging efforts.

Methods

We used data collected from GPS loggers on golden-mantled ground squirrels (Callospermophilus lateralis) to model selection or avoidance of resources in two seasons of seed availability and one season in which no seeds were available. Movement decisions were measured in short-term discrete time intervals using high resolution location data. Selection or avoidance of specific resource features that entail fitness consequences were then assessed using resource selection functions.

Results

Seasonality of food availability, food type, and spatial distribution of food largely influenced how individuals selected resources within their home ranges. Overall, when seeds were available, individuals mediated risks of predation and loss of food by using patches closer to refuge and selected intermediate distances to the burrow. When food was not available, individuals minimized exposure to heightened risk by staying close to the burrow and avoiding riskier patch types.

Conclusions

Results indicate that individuals used flexible, dynamic strategies to select habitat patches which may allow them to balance conflicting seasonal demands. Advances in GPS technology for research of small mammals provide greater insight into how prey species in high risk environments differentially use resources to minimize risk and maintain fitness.

Background

All animals experience dynamic physiological and environmental demands and must adjust their activity patterns to minimize associated costs and increase fitness. Fulfilling energetic requirements, limiting competitive interactions, avoiding predation, searching for mates, and minimizing exposure to unfavorable abiotic elements are some of the competing demands that influence how species use resources within their environment [1]. The level of risk (i.e. cost to fitness) individuals perceive in response to these demands is not temporally or spatially constant, and often risk is compounded by simultaneously occurring demands [1, 2]. To appropriately respond to these dynamic conditions, individuals must differentially use movement paths and select resources that will minimize perceived risk.

Appropriate methods to analyze how individuals perceive and respond to risk associated with conflicting demands remain debated in literature [3, 4]. Many studies have used experimental methods to understand bilateral trade-offs in behavioral responses [5, 6]. For example, giving-up densities (GUDs) measure trade-offs between behaviors such as foraging effort and predator avoidance by measuring density of food left in a patch or experimental food trays as an indicator of the cost of foraging under predation risk [1, 7, 8]. GUDs assume perceived predation risk is greater with increasing amounts of food left by an individual, indicating the point at which the cost of foraging exceeds the energetic benefits. This approach has been criticized for its assumption that individuals assess food consistently among patches in time and space and disregards additional extrinsic and intrinsic factors that also may influence an individual’s decision to leave a patch [4, 5, 9, 10].

Animal movement is inherently driven by extrinsic and intrinsic factors. Patterns in movement reveal how individuals select resources to fulfill energetic requirements, minimize exposure to costly abiotic elements, and reduce predation and mortality risk [11, 12]. It is well documented that wildlife species will alter corridor use, daily activity patterns, and migration routes to minimize risk [12,13,14]. Recent advances in GPS technology and analysis methods have allowed for detailed inference regarding animal movement. These methods can provide valuable information regarding decisions individuals make in heterogeneous environments and how those decisions may change dependent on dynamic conditions within their environments. For example, differential selection of resources driven by food quality and quantity within and between seasons may be evident as shifts in movement paths between seasons as individuals select different habitat patches [15, 16]. Likewise, avoidance of a perceived risk, such as predator threat, may be evident as a reduction in density of location data in space or time where perceived risk is highest [17]. Extrinsic risks can be mediated by selecting safer corridors (i.e. corridors with ready access to refuge) or by modifications in individual behavior, such as periodic episodes of antipredator vigilance [3, 17, 18]. Given the intrinsic variability and uncertainty in heterogeneous environments, this flexibility in resource selection and behavior is critical for individual survival.

Golden-mantled ground squirrels (Callospermophilus lateralis) are an excellent study species to analyze how individuals use differential selection of resources to balance dynamic, conflicting demands. C. lateralis are ubiquitous in granivore-driven ecosystems in mountainous regions of western North America. Like other central-place foragers, C. lateralis must minimize the energetic cost of foraging while maximizing energy gained from food gathered [19, 20]. Additionally, C. lateralis are solitary, and individuals must minimize costs associated with dynamically changing demands such as predator pressure, exposure to abiotic elements in unsuitable matrices, and competitive interactions [21,22,23]. Adjusting activity patterns and movement paths is crucial to balance these risks, particularly in a system where food availability is seasonal and unpredictable year to year due to annual drastic variation in seed crop abundance [24, 25]. C. lateralis use a larder-hoarding strategy, in which they place all their seeds in a burrow and defend that burrow from sympatric granivore species [26, 27]. Competing yellow-pine chipmunks (Tamias amoenus) scatter-hoard seeds and use their acute olfactory sense to pilfer from caches made by conspecifics and other species [28]. C. lateralis cannot reciprocate this pilfering behavior, which makes larder-defense their primary strategy to store food [27, 28]. Despite the importance of defending the larder, individuals must often forego larder defense to participate in other essential activities, including foraging. When individuals leave the burrow, they not only leave the larder vulnerable to competitors, but also incur greater levels of direct physical risk, such as predation. Therefore, C. lateralis must effectively evaluate and appropriately respond to competing risks to maximize fitness.

Herein, we address how C. lateralis use flexible strategies to balance conflicting demands of predator avoidance, larder-defense, and foraging. Resource selection functions (RSFs) were used to identify differential selection of resources between seasons of seed availability and one season when seeds were not available. Though, RSFs have most commonly been used in studies of large mammals, they can be very helpful in determining resource use by small mammals that experience unique habitat constraints [29,30,31,32]. Assuming an overwhelming necessity of needing to forage in an environment of exceptionally unpredictable and limited food availability, we predicted that: 1) In all seasons, C. lateralis should select foraging sites closer to the burrow to protect the larder from pilferers and to reduce the risk of predation, 2) During seasons of seed availability, C. lateralis should select patches in which probable encounter rate with food is highest, and 3) When individuals are away from the burrow, they should remain within close proximity to points of refuge, such as boulders and stumps, to escape predators.

Methods

Study site

Our study area was located in the semiarid eastern Sierra Nevada, where seed availability is ephemeral and particularly limited [33, 34]. We conducted the study within a 0.43 km2 area in the Whittell Forest and Wildlife Area in Little Valley, Washoe County, 30 km south of Reno, Nevada, USA (39°15′0″N, 119°52′35″W). This study site is owned by the University of Nevada, Reno, and comprises 1073 ha at an average elevation of 1975 m. Dominant woody vegetation includes Jeffrey pine (Pinus jeffreyi), lodgepole pine (Pinus contorta), antelope bitterbrush (Purshia tridentata), greenleaf manzanita (Arctostaphylos patula), tobacco bush (Ceanothus velutinus), and Sierra bush chinquapin (Castanopsis sempervirens). The portion of Little Valley selected for this study had a dominant open tree canopy of Jeffrey pine and a patchy understory of antelope bitterbrush. Those two species of plants were the only seed-producing plants in the site to support populations of C. lateralis and granivorous competitors. Seeds are the dominant source of food for granivores in this study site and therefore dependence on this food source is much higher than other scarce opportunistic encounters with other food sources. Bitterbrush seeds, when available in July, were restricted to stands of shrubs existing primarily in clumped distribution beneath open (e.g., no overstory cover) canopy. Jeffrey pine seeds were randomly scattered by the wind in early September. Availability of both bitterbrush and Jeffrey pine seeds was abundant during the time of the study. Primary predators of C. lateralis during the summer and fall in this region primarily consisted of birds-of-prey.

Animal capture and handling

During July–September, 2014, 9 adult C. lateralis (five males and four females) were selected for study (Table 1). Most individuals were used more than once, dependent on researcher ability to consistently recapture those individuals. Squirrels were captured in Tomahawk model #102 live traps (Tomahawk Live Trap Co., Hazelhurst, Wisconsin) and handled in accordance with a protocol approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (UNR IACUC, #A07/08–30) that was in keeping with guidelines established by the American Society of Mammalogists for use of wild mammals in research [35]. GiPSy 5 global positioning system (GPS) loggers (5 g, 17 × 12 × 4 mm) supplied by TechnoSmart Europe Srl (Colleverde, Italy), and very high frequency (VHF) transmitters (0.8 g) supplied by Advanced Telemetry Systems Inc. (Isanti, Minnesota) were used to track individuals and retrieve them. Telemetry transmitters were epoxied to the side of each GPS logger, and loggers were attached to squirrels using zip-ties threaded through hollow fabric cords. Individuals on average weighed 165 ± 24 g.

Table 1 Capture, home range size, and GPS fix summary for individuals used in a 2014 study of resource selection of golden-mantled ground squirrels (Callospermophilus lateralis) in the Whittell Forest and Wildlife Area, NV, USA. Season represents differing availability of food items for individuals

Data were collected in three different seasons. Those seasons were delineated by timing and type of seeds that were available: 1) bitterbrush season (bitterbrush seeds available on shrubs July 21–31, 2014), 2) interval season (no plants producing seeds, August 1–September 11, 2014), and 3) pine season (Jeffrey pine seeds available, September 12–19, 2014). GPS loggers were programmed to collect fixes on one of two schedules: 5 rapid fixes (i.e. 1 fix per second for 5 s) at 5 min intervals or 1 fix every 10 s. The 10 s fix collection was initially used to gain more accurate locations per fix, but was subsequently changed to every five minutes with rapid fixes to save battery and maintain accuracy by averaging the locations of the five rapid fixes. On these schedules, the battery lasted for 3–4 days. After that time, loggers were retrieved from the squirrels and data were downloaded.

Habitat modeling

All GPS locations collected from all individuals were used to create 95% fixed kernel density home range polygons using the Geospatial Modelling Environment [36]. Home range estimates were smoothed using the least squares cross-validation (LSCV), which is associated with the least biased estimates of home range size, particularly with smaller sample sizes [37]. All home range polygons were imported into ArcMap 10.3 and inspected for accuracy and to ensure outlying points did not cause unrealistic elongation of the estimated home range.

A vegetation raster with 1-m resolution was created in ArcMap 10.3 that described three dominant canopy cover types: open, pine, and antelope bitterbrush. Open canopy was categorized by no overstory or understory vegetative cover. Pine canopy was categorized by no bitterbrush understory and pine as an overstory cover. Bitterbrush canopy was categorized by a dominant understory cover of bitterbrush, regardless of whether there was an overstory cover of pine or not. Open and pine canopy cover were defined via digitization using 1-m resolution imagery from the National Agriculture Imagery Program. Our study site was classified as 5% primary bitterbrush canopy cover, 50% Jeffrey pine cover, and 45% open canopy. In the field, bitterbrush cover, burrows, and refuge locations were mapped at the study site using a Trimble GeoExplorer 7x. Bitterbrush shrubs were mapped as a continuous stand if < 1 m apart. Refuge locations consisted of stumps, boulders, and fallen logs under which rodent tunnels were located. These locations were mapped as either points (< 1 m in diameter) or polygons (> 1 m in diameter). If refuge locations such as boulders were < 1 m apart, they were mapped as a continuous polygon. Distance to the nearest refuge area was calculated using the Near tool in ArcMap 10.2 [38]. Separate rasters were created for canopy cover and distance to the home burrow. Distance from the home burrow was represented using a Euclidean distance raster with each burrow as the point of origination.

Resource selection functions (RSFs) were used to determine the probability of selection or avoidance of resources [39]. RSFs are often used to inform movement connectivity analyses and are flexible in their ability to analyze changes in habitat selection over time. Rather than using a movement-based model such as a step selection function, we chose to implement the RSF to model resource use because we deemed it a more appropriate method to accurately measure macroscopic space use while considering the small spatial scale used by study individuals, observed above-ground behavior of individuals, and the nature of the unequal sample intervals between fix locations [40]. To account for the assumption made by RSFs that the study area is entirely available to each individual at any time step, we selected individuals whose home ranges did not include seasonal or long-term barriers to movement as well as areas in which resources could feasibly be exploited year-round by each individual at any time [41]. Field observations and live trap grids were used to determine resource accessibility for each individual prior to the study. To create the resource selection function, categories of used and available points were designated [42, 43]. Locations of squirrels obtained from GPS data loggers represented habitat used by each squirrel (used points), while habitat available to that squirrel was represented by creating a set of random points to represent availability. Random points were generated within each home range for each squirrel generally at a 1:3 ratio of used to randomly generated points. Random points were generated so that they proportionally represented the vegetative cover types within the home range. When used points were few (< 60) or random points did not proportionally represent the underlying vegetation within the individual’s home range, we increased the number of random points to 1:5 ratio to appropriately characterize available resources. Raster values for distance to burrow, distance to refuge, and vegetative canopy cover type (i.e. open, bitterbrush shrub cover, or pine tree cover) were extracted for both used and random points. Non-parametric Kruskal-Wallis tests were used to analyze differences among distance to burrow and distance to refuge between seasons. A post-hoc Dunn’s test was used to test pairwise comparisons between seasons.

Resource selection was evaluated for all individuals at the scale of the home range for each individual (third order scale) [39]. To represent variation in selection of resources among seasons, we created three different mixed-effect logistic regression models, one per season [44,45,46]. For each season, we modeled canopy cover type as a categorical covariate with three levels (pine, open, and bitterbrush) and distance to refuge and distance to the burrow as continuous covariates. All statistics were calculated using the lme4 package in R version 3.1.2 [47,48,49]. Individual squirrels were modeled as a random effect to account for variation in number of locations collected among individuals [47, 48]. Non-linearity was graphically inspected for continuous variables using frequency histograms. When non-linearity was suspected, continuous variables were represented as both linear and non-linear (quadratic) terms [50]. Model selection was performed using Akaike Information criterion adjusted for small sample sizes (AICc) [51]. We examined models closely for the presence of uninformative parameters and selected the model which best fit our data [52, 53].

Additionally, continuous variables were standardized to allow for direct comparison among parameter estimates [54, 55]. For the discrete variable of canopy type, pine canopy was chosen as the intercept because it comprised the majority of the study area. Additionally, most individuals make their burrows under open canopy and would not use pine canopy as shelter while trying to avoid a predator [56]. For discrete variables (i.e. vegetative canopy cover) resulting positive model coefficients indicated selection of a covariate while negative values indicated avoidance of a covariate [55]. For continuous variables representing distance to a resource (i.e. refuge or the burrow), negative model coefficients indicate selection for shorter travel distances because probability of selection decreases as distance to a point of interest increases. Alternatively, positive coefficients indicate avoidance of a resource of interest because probability of selection increases as distance from a resource increases.

Results

All seasons were significantly different from one another for both distance to burrow (Kruskal-Wallis chi-squared = 180.36, df = 2, p-value< 0.001) and distance to refuge (Kruskal-Wallis chi-squared = 242.06, df = 2, p-value< 0.001). In general, individuals traveled farthest from the burrow during the bitterbrush season (66 ± 52 m) and remained relatively close to the burrow during the interval season (48 ± 26 m) and Jeffrey pine season (46 ± 57 m). Individuals were active closer to refuge locations on average during the bitterbrush season (9 ± 9 m) and Jeffrey pine season (7 ± 16 m) and were active farther from refuge locations during the interval season (12 ± 13 m). Therefore, separate models were run for each season.

Resource selection

While all seasons included multiple models with Δ AICc scores< 2, some of these top models included uninformative parameters [51, 52]. During both the bitterbrush seed and interval seasons, there were multiple models with Δ AICc scores< 2, but the additional parameters in lower ranked models were uninformative and did not improve model fit. The pine season had four models with a delta AIC < 2, with the fourth ranked model containing uninformative parameters. Since the remaining top three models all contained informative parameters and parameters in the top ranked model were not a subset of lower ranked models, the top three models were averaged. In some models across seasons, distance to the burrow or refuge was best modeled as a quadratic relationship which indicated selection of an intermediate distance from the burrow or points of refuge. The top model for the bitterbrush season included a quadratic relationship for distance to burrow (linear term: β = − 1.5257 SE = 0.0891, squared term: β = 0.7973 SE = 0.0558), selection for bitterbrush canopy cover (β = 0.5929 SE = 0.1510), and selection for open canopy cover (β = 0.7487 SE = 0.0537) (Fig. 1). Additionally, individuals used locations closer to refuge areas than predicted by availability (β = − 0.7318 SE = 0.0558). During the season in which no seeds were available, the top model included a quadratic relationship for distance to refuge areas (linear term: β = 0.058 SE = 0.1638, squared term: β = − 0.1623 SE = 0.0686), avoidance of bitterbrush canopy cover (β = − 0.2373 SE = 0.0647), and avoidance of open canopy cover (β = − 0.1494 SE = 0.0439). Additionally, individuals used locations closer to the burrow than predicted by availability (β = − 0.8450 SE = 0.0298). The three models averaged for pine season all contained quadratic relationships for distance to burrow (linear term: β = − 4.3109 SE = 0.1928, squared term: β = 3.2553 SE = 0.1693). The top model additionally included a quadratic relationship for distance to refuge areas (linear term: β = − 0.2534 SE = 0.1719, squared term: β = 0.2951 SE = 0.1890).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Model generated beta estimates with 85% confidence intervals from an analysis of resource selection by Callospermophilus lateralis relative to three seasons of food availability: bitterbrush, interval (no seeds available), and pine. Selection for parameters along the y-axis are represented by positive values along the x-axis whereas avoidance of parameters along the y-axis are represented by negative values along the x-axis. Study was conducted July 15–September 30, 2014, in the Whittell Forest and Wildlife Area, NV, USA

Distance to the burrow was best modeled as a quadratic relationship for both seasons that seeds were available. For both seasons, this quadratic relationship indicated higher use of locations closest to the burrow compared to available locations (Fig. 2). Additionally, during bitterbrush season this quadratic function indicated increasing use of locations at intermediate distances from the burrow compared to available locations. For pine season, this function indicated a slight increase in use of locations farthest from the burrow, although overall individuals remained closer to the burrow. Quadratic relationships for distance to refuge areas were also observed for the season of no seed availability and pine season. When plotted, it was evident that use was highest at locations close to refuge areas compared to available locations (Fig. 3).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Density plots of GPS logged points from Callospermophilus lateralis depicting distance traveled from the burrow (m) during three seasons of food availability: a) Bitterbrush, b) Interval (no seed available), and c) Pine. The dark gray indicates used points (gained from GPS loggers) and the light gray indicates randomly generated available points. Study was conducted July 15–September 30, 2014, in the Whittell Forest and Wildlife Area, NV, USA

Fig. 3
figure 3

Density plots of GPS logged points from Callospermophilus lateralis depicting distance traveled from the refuge (m) during two seasons of food availability: a) Bitterbrush and b) Interval (no seed available), and c) Pine. The dark gray indicates used points (gained from GPS loggers) and the light gray indicates randomly generated available points. Study was conducted July 15–September 30, 2014, in the Whittell Forest and Wildlife Area, NV, USA

During bitterbrush season, individuals selected both open and bitterbrush canopy cover, though they selected open canopy cover slightly more than bitterbrush. During the season of no seed availability, individuals avoided both open and bitterbrush canopy cover. During pine season, individuals showed no significant selection for or avoidance of either open or bitterbrush canopy cover relative to pine canopy cover.

Discussion

The ability to balance conflicting demands is key to surviving in highly competitive and risky landscapes [13, 20, 57]. For C. lateralis, this balance means optimizing foraging efforts to meet energetic requirements while also minimizing the risk of predation and loss of food stores to pilferers. Individuals modified travel distances from the burrow and points of refuge, as well as differentially selected vegetative canopy cover by season that could be indicative of individuals attempting to balance these conflicting demands. Contrary to our first prediction, during seasons of seed availability there was evidence of use of locations at intermediate and farther distances from the burrow, in contrast to the season of no seed availability when individuals selected distances closer to the burrow. As predicted, selection for proximity to refuge occurred during seasons of seed availability when individuals foraged away from the burrow. Finally, selection for vegetative canopy cover reflected seed availability by season.

Risk of starvation and predation are two of the greatest threats to the survival of C. lateralis as well as other small rodents [58]. For central-place foraging species, the burrow acts as a protective shelter from predators [59,60,61], but for C. lateralis, proximity to the burrow also is important to protect the larder from pilferers (personal observation, KLH). When and how individuals choose to maintain foraging activity in risky environments (i.e. at great distances from the burrow) is dependent on seed availability and individual energy requirements [62,63,64]. In circumstances when energy requirements are high, the need to forage may even supersede predator avoidance [65, 66]. While a quadratic relationship best represented distance traveled from the burrow for both seasons of seed availability, there was variation in the proximity of those distances traveled from the burrow between seasons (see Fig. 2). In particular, individuals displayed strong selection of intermediate locations (100–200 m) away from the burrow in comparison to pine season. Bitterbrush is the first seed to become available in the summer and individuals are obligated to travel from the burrow to bitterbrush patches where seeds are available.

In contrast, during pine season individuals generally remained closest to the burrow (see Fig. 2). During years with high pine mast, pine seeds predominate other seed species in T. amoenus winter larders in Little Valley [25], indicating these seeds are highly valuable for winter survival. Unlike bitterbrush seeds, pine seeds are dispersed randomly by wind events and individuals are not obligated to travel long distances if they encounter seeds closer to the burrow. Pilfering pressure may also be higher on individuals at this time during the year, as the larder becomes larger and more valuable [25, 34]. In a study testing the pilfering rates of red squirrels (Tamiasciurus hudsonicus), squirrels with larger middens were pilfered from at a higher rate than squirrels with smaller middens [67]. Individuals may return to their burrows for shelter during bitterbrush season rather than to defend the larder, then switch to a larder-defense strategy during pine season when the larder is more valuable. Although some exceptionally long trips from the burrow were logged during pine season, these trips may likely be explained by the increased value of pine seeds late in the year. When seeds nearest to the burrow become depleted, it becomes necessary to make long trips to accrue enough food to survive winter torpor.

Small mammal species must adopt resource use strategies that balance foraging and predator avoidance [22, 68]. This balance may best be achieved for C. lateralis by concentrating activity at locations nearer to the burrow when possible or at intermediate distances from the burrow when seed sources are patchily distributed as in bitterbrush season. Following bitterbrush season, individuals may simply be less inclined to travel frequently from the burrow when encounter rate with seeds has drastically decreased. Consistent with optimal foraging theory, a forager’s harvest rate should decrease as resources in a patch become depleted [69].

If individuals are obligated to travel farther from the burrow to gather seeds, proximity to refuge locations becomes important to reduce predation risk. Although there are certainly more refuge areas than were detected in the study, overall, proximity to locations of refuge appeared to allow individuals to maximize their time foraging while simultaneously reducing their risk of predation. Many other central-place foraging species have been shown to forage near areas of cover when away from the nest or burrow [22, 68]. As explained by previous studies using giving-up densities, animals are less likely to abandon foraging entirely in risky landscapes if they have ready access to refuge [70,71,72,73]. This allows individuals to meet their energetic requirements while dually minimizing predation risk.

Proximity to refuge also may enable individuals to use riskier corridors. Although it may seem surprising that individuals select open canopy cover during the bitterbrush season, there are several reasons why individuals may want to use open canopy cover types while foraging. In particular, antelope bitterbrush grows most commonly in shrub stands with little to no overstory cover [74]. Additionally, traveling through open canopy is also more cost effective and may allow them to return to bitterbrush patches and harvest seeds more quickly [75]. Larger-bodied members of the granivore guild have even been shown to exploit riskier open areas to avoid competitive exclusion and are able to do so because of their increased locomotive ability [76]. Unlike their smaller-bodied competitors, larger desert rodents have been shown to select open canopy foraging patches of higher predation risk if the quality of seed is high [77]. This differential selection of foraging patches also promotes coexistence between species within the granivore guild [78].

When seeds were not available, individuals no longer selected habitat patches beneath open canopy (see Fig. 1). When individuals are less active in foraging away from the burrow, they should conserve energy, protect their larder, and avoid predation. During pine season, bitterbrush and open canopy were neither selected nor avoided but used in proportion to their availability. Pine seeds are scattered at random by the wind, so seed encounter rate could be just as likely near to the burrow as well as farther from the burrow until availability of seeds near the burrow are depleted.

The conclusions made in this study are limited by small sample sizes, but to our knowledge this is the first study to use GPS data loggers on species with such a small body size. Nevertheless, this study has shown that movement data can still have great explanatory power when uncovering how individuals make decisions to reduce risk and how these decisions changed as related to changes within their habitat. This is particularly relevant to species for which basic ecological data is already known, such as C. lateralis. Additionally, while there are great benefits to using GPS technology, it is many times more expensive than traditional VHF telemetry. Additionally, as occurred in this study, burrowing animals present unique challenges to both GPS and telemetry equipment. Antennas can snap off in tunnels and animals can lose the logger altogether while squeezing through narrow places. Loggers were shed inside burrow tunnels on three occasions and were retrieved if they still emitted a VHF signal. If the telemetry antenna was snapped off, the logger could not be tracked and retrieved. Two study animals disappeared from the site, mostly likely due to predation events, though a logger from one individual was found on top of a boulder inside the study site. Overall, from field observation, loggers did not appear to inhibit movement nor behavior of individuals in the field. Normal foraging activity, grooming, and pursuit of competitors was still observed.

Conclusions

Use of high-resolution location data allowed for a more accurate understanding of movement decisions C. lateralis makes in response to fluctuations of food availability and dynamic stressors. When applied to studies of small mammals, these data provide promising unbiased evidence of individual movements decisions as influenced by intrinsic and extrinsic demands. Using GPS technology allows animals the ability to behave normally without the interruption of researchers actively tracking them. GPS also saves time in the field, which can ultimately save money in travel expenditures. As GPS equipment becomes smaller in size, more affordable, and thus more accessible for small mammal studies, conducting such analyses for small mammals should be more feasible.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

References

  1. Brown JS, Kotler BP. Hazardous duty pay and the foraging cost of predation. Ecol Lett. 2004;7:999–1014.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Camp MJ, Shipley LA, Johnson TR, Olsoy PJ, Forbey JS, Rachlow JL, et al. The balancing act of foraging: mammalian herbivores trade-off multiple risks when selecting food patches. Oecologia. 2017;185:537–49.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Carthey AJ, Banks PB. Foraging in groups affects giving-up densities: solo foragers quit sooner. Oecologia. 2015;178:707–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. McMahon JD, Lashley MA, Brooks CP, Barton BT. Covariance between predation risk and nutritional preferences confounds interpretations of giving-up density experiments. Ecology. 2018;99:1517–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Verdolin JL. Meta-analysis of foraging and predation risk trade-offs in terrestrial systems. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2006;60:457–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Dehn MM, Ydenberg RC, Dill LM. Experimental addition of cover lowers the perception of danger and increases reproduction in meadow voles (Microtus pennsylvanicus). Can J Zoolog. 2017;95:463–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Brown JS. Patch use as an indicator of habitat preference, predation risk and competition. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1988;22:37–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Bedoya-Perez MA, Carthey AJR, Mella VSA, McArthur C, Banks PB. A practical guide to avoid giving up on giving up densities. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2013;87:1541–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Nonacs P. State dependent behavior and the marginal value theorem. Behav Ecol. 2001;12:71–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Price MV, Correll RA. Depletion of seed patches by Merriam’s kangaroo rats: are GUD assumptions met? Ecol Lett. 2001;4:334–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Kittle AM, Fryxell DGE, Hamr J. The scale-dependent impact of wolf predation risk on resource selection by three sympatric ungulates. Oecologia. 2008;157:163–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Raynor EJ, Beyer HL, Briggs JM, Joern A. Complex variation in habitat selection strategies among individuals driven by extrinsic factors. Ecol Evol. 2016;7:1802–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Lima SL. Maximizing feeding efficiency and minimizing time exposed to predators: a trade-off in the black-capped chickadee. Oecologia. 1985;66:60–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Leblond M, St-Laurent M-H, Côté SD. Caribou, water, and ice – fine-scale movements of a migratory arctic ungulate in the context of climate change. Mov Ecol. 2016;4:14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Matthews A. Changes in fine-scale movement and foraging patterns of common wombats along a snow-depth gradient. Wildlife Res. 2010;37:175–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Van Beest FM, Mysterud A, Loe LE, Milner JM. Forage quantity, quality and depletion as scale-dependent mechanisms driving habitat selection of a large browsing herbivore. J Anim Ecol. 2010;79:910–22.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Brinkerhoff RJ, Haddad NM, Orrock JL. Corridors and olfactory predator cues affect small mammal behavior. J Mammal. 2005;86:662–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Kotler BP. Behavioral resource depression and decaying perceived risk of predation in two species of coexisting gerbils. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1992;30:239–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. MacArthur RH, Pianka ER. On optimal use of a patchy environment. Am Nat. 1966;100:603–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Pyke GH, Pulliam HR, Charnov EL. Optimal foraging: a selective review of theory and tests. Q Rev Biol. 1977;52:137–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Ferron J. Social behavior of the golden-mantled ground squirrel (Spermophilus lateralis). Can J Zoolog. 1985;63:2529–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Bonenfant M, Kramer DL. The influence of distance to burrow on flight initiation distance in the woodchuck, Marmota monax. Behav Ecol. 1996;7:299–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Fuller AK, Harrison DJ. Movement paths reveal scale-dependent habitat decisions by Canada lynx. J Mammal. 2010;91:1269–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Vander Wall SB. Masting in animal-dispersed pines facilitates seed dispersal. Ecology. 2002;83:3508–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Kuhn KM, Vander Wall SB. Linking summer foraging to winter survival in yellow pine chipmunks (Tamias amoenus). Oecologia. 2008;157:349–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Vander Wall SB. The influence of environmental conditions on cache recovery and cache pilferage by yellow pine chipmunks (Tamias amoenus) and deer mice (Peromyscus maniculatus). Behav Ecol. 2000;11:544–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Vander Wall SB, Enders MS, Waitman BA. Asymmetrical cache pilfering between yellow pine chipmunks and golden-mantled ground squirrels. Anim Behav. 2009;78:555–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Wang ZY, Wang B, Yi X, Yan C, Cao L, Zhang Z. Scatter-hoarding rodents are better pilferers than larder-hoarders. Anim Behav. 2018;141:151–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Hough MJ, Dieter CD. Resource selection habitat model for northern flying squirrels in the Black Hills, South Dakota. Am Midl Nat. 2009;162:356–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Barker OE, Derocher AE. Habitat selection by arctic ground squirrels (Spermophilus parryii). J Mammal. 2010;91:1251–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Perkins MW, Conner LM. Habitat use of fox squirrels in southwestern Georgia. J Wildlife Manage. 2004;68:509–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Doumas SL, Koprowski JL. Effect of heterogeneity in burn severity on Mexican fox squirrels following the return of fire. Int J Wildland Fire. 2013;22:405–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Simberloff D, Dayan T. The guild concept and the structure of ecological communities. Annu Rev Ecol Syst. 1991;22:115–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Vander Wall SB. Seed fate pathways of antelope bitterbrush: dispersal by seed-caching yellow pine chipmunks. Ecology. 1994;75:1911–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Sikes RS, Animal Care and Use Committee of the American Society of Mammalogists. 2016 guidelines of the American Society of Mammalogists for the use of wild mammals in research and education. J Mammal 2016 09;97:663–688.

  36. Beyer HL. The geospatial modelling environment version 0; 2012. p. 6.0.0.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Seaman DE. 1999. Effects of sample size on kernel home range estimates. J Wildlife Manage. 1999;63:739–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. ESRI 2012. ArcGIS Desktop: Release 10.2. Redlands, CA: Environmental Systems Research Institute.

  39. Manly BFJ, McDonald LL, Thomas DL, McDonald TL, Erickson WP. Resource selection by animals: statistical design and analysis for field studies. 2nd edn, vol. 222. New York: Kluwer Press; 2002.

    Google Scholar 

  40. Michelot T, Blackwell PG, Matthiopoulos J. Linking resource selection and step selection models for habitat preferences in animals. Ecology. 2018;100:e02452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Beyer HL, Haydon DT, Morales JM, Frair JL, Hebblewhite M, Mitchell M, Matthiopoulos J. 2010. The interpretation of habitat preference metrics under use–availability designs. Philos Trans R Soc B. 2010;365:2245–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Long RA, Bowyer RT, Porter WP, Mathewson P, Monteith KL, Kie JG. Behavior and nutritional condition buffer a large-bodied endotherm against direct and indirect effects of climate. Ecol Monogr. 2014;84:513–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Mckee CJ, Stewart KM, Sedinger JS, Bush AP, Darby NW, Hughson DL, et al. Spatial distributions and resource selection by mule deer in an arid environment: responses to provision of water. J Arid Environ. 2015;122:76–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Nielsen SE, Boyce MS, Stenhouse GB, Munroe RHM. Development and testing of phenologically driven grizzly bear habitat models. EcoScience. 2003;10, 10(1).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Nielsen SE, Boyce MS, Stenhouse GB. Grizzly bears and forestry. I: Selection of clearcuts by grizzly bears in west-central Alberta, Canada For Ecol Manage. 2004;199:51–65.

    Google Scholar 

  46. Boyce MS. Scale for resource selection functions. Divers Distrib. 2006;12:269–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. R Development Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2011. https://www.r-project.org/. Accessed 3 Mar 2016.

  48. Gillies CS, Hebblewhite M, Nielsen SE, Krawchuk MA, Aldridge CL, Frair JL, et al. Application of random effects to the study of resource selection by animals. J Anim Ecol. 2006;75:887–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Bolker BM, Brooks ME, Clark CJ, Geange SW, Poulsen JR, Henry MHH, et al. Generalized linear mixed models: a practical guide for ecology and evolution. Trends Ecol Evol. 2009;24:127–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Hosmer Jr. DW, Lemeshow S, Sturdivant RX. Applied logistic regression. 3rd ed. New York: John Wiley and Sons; 2013. 528 p.

  51. Burnham KP, Anderson DR. Model selection and multimodel inference: a practical information-theoretic approach. 2nd ed. New York: Springer; 2002. 488 p

    Google Scholar 

  52. Arnold TW. Uninformative parameters and model selection using Akaike’s information criterion. J Wildlife Manage. 2010;74:1175–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Aho K, Derryberry D, Peterson T. A graphical framework for model selection criteria and significance tests: refutation, confirmation, and ecology. Methods Ecol Evol. 2017;8:47–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Neter JM, Kutner MH, Wasserman W, Nachtsheim CJ. Applied linear statistical models. 4th ed. New York: McGraw-Hill/Irwin; 1996. p. 1408.

    Google Scholar 

  55. Stewart KM, Walsh DR, Kie JG, Dick BL, Bowyer RT. Sexual segregation in north American elk: the role of density dependence. Ecol Evol. 2015;5:709–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Shick KR, Pearson DE, Ruggiero LF. Forest habitat associations of the golden-mantled ground squirrel: implications for fuels management. Northwest Sci. 2006;80:133–9.

    Google Scholar 

  57. Rothley KD, Schmitz OJ, Cohon JL. Foraging to balance conflicting demands: novel insights from grasshoppers under predation risk. Behav Ecol. 1997;8:551–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Lima SL, Dill LM. Behavioral decisions made under the risk of predation: a review and prospectus. Can J Zoolog. 1990;68:619–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Andino N, Borghi CE, Giannoni SM. Characterization and selection of microhabitat of Microcavia australis (Rodentia: Caviidae): first data in a rocky habitat in the hyperarid Monte Desert of Argentina. Mammalia. 2016;80:71–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Crowell MM, Shipley LA, Camp MJ, Rachlow JL, Forbey JS, Johnson TR. Selection of food patches by sympatric herbivores in response to concealment and distance from a refuge. Ecol Evol. 2016;6:2865–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Lagos PA, Meier A, Ortiz L, Castro RA, Bozinovic F, Ebensperger LA. Flight initiation distance is differentially sensitive to the costs of staying and leaving food patches in a small-mammal prey. Can J Zoolog. 2009;87:1016–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Kotler BP, Brown JS, Bouskila A. Apprehension and time allocation in gerbils: the effects of predatory risk and energetic state. Ecology. 2004;85:917–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Lima SL. Initiation and termination of daily feeding in dark-eyed juncos: influences of predation risk and energy reserves. Oikos. 1998;53:3–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Suselbeek L, Emsens WJ, Hirsch BT, Kays R, Rowcliffe JM, Zamora-Gutierrez V, et al. Food acquisition and predator avoidance in a neotropical rodent. Anim Behav. 2014;88:41–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Anholt BR, Werner EE. Interaction between food availability and predation mortality mediated by adaptive behavior. Ecology. 1995;76:2230–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Hilton GM, Ruxton GD, Cresswell W. Choice of foraging area with respect to predation risk in redshanks: the effects of weather and predator activity. Oikos. 1999;87:295–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Gerhardt F. Food pilfering in larder-hoarding red squirrels (Tamiasciurus hudsonicus). J Mammal. 2005;86:108–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Thorson JM, Morgan RA, Brown JS, Norman JE. Direct and indirect cues of predatory risk and patch use by fox squirrels and thirteen-lined ground squirrels. Behav Ecol. 1997;9:151–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Brown JS. Vigilance, patch use and habitat selection: foraging under predation risk. Evol Ecol Res. 1998;1:49–71.

    Google Scholar 

  70. Brown JS, Morgan RA, Dow BD. Patch use under predation risk: II. A test with fox squirrels, Sciurus niger. Ann Zool Fenn. 1992;29:311–8.

    Google Scholar 

  71. Bowers MA, Jefferson JL, Kuebler MG. Variation in giving-up densities of foraging chipmunks (Tamias striatus) and squirrels (Sciurus carolinensis). Oikos. 1993;66:229–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Brown JS, Morgan RA. Effects of foraging behavior and spatial scale on diet selectivity-a test with fox squirrels. Oikos. 1995;74:122–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Johnson MD, De León YL. Effect of an invasive plant and moonlight on rodent foraging behavior in a coastal dune ecosystem. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0117903.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Dyer D. USDA & NRCS Plant Guide: Antelope Bitterbrush (Purshia tridentata (Pursh) DC. [Internet]. Lockeford, CA: USDA NRCS Plant Materials Center; 2014 [updated 2014 July 8; cited 2016 July 22]. Available from: https://plants.usda.gov/plantguide/pdf/pg_putr2.pdf

  75. Kenagy GJ, Hoyt DF. Speed and time-energy budget for locomotion in golden-mantled ground squirrels. Ecology. 1989;70:1834–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Smith RJ. Harvest rates and escape speeds in 2 coexisting species of montane ground-squirrels. J Mammal. 1995;76:189–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Kotler BP. Risk of predation and the structure of desert rodent communities. Ecology. 1984;65:689–701.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Schoener TW. Resource partitioning in ecological communities. Science. 1974;185:27–39.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Several fellow graduate students and faculty members at the University of Nevada, Reno, contributed to field efforts and the revision of this work. We would particularly like to thank Dr. Stephen B. Vander Wall, Dr. Marjorie D. Matocq, Dr. William S. Longland, Dr. Jacob W. Dittel, Thomas Dilts, Meredith Reia, and Jeffrey M. Gicklhorn.

Funding

KLH received supported from the University of Nevada, Reno, Whittell Forest Graduate Fellowship endowed fund. This fund aided with the purchase of GPS equipment and fuel for transportation to and from the study site.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

KLH conceived, designed, and executed this study and wrote the manuscript. KMS provided expertise and editorial support. Both authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Kira L. Hefty.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The author’s declare they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hefty, K.L., Stewart, K.M. Flexible resource use strategies of a central-place forager experiencing dynamic risk and opportunity. Mov Ecol 7, 23 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40462-019-0168-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40462-019-0168-2

Keywords