Skip to main content

Factors modulating home range and resource use: a case study with Canarian houbara bustards

Abstract

Background

The home range of an animal is determined by its ecological requirements, and these may vary depending on many intrinsic and extrinsic factors, which are ultimately driven by food resources. Investigating the effects of these factors, and specifically how individuals use food resources within their home ranges is essential to understand the ecology and dynamics of animal populations, and to establish conservation measures in the case of endangered species. Here, we investigate these questions in the Canarian houbara bustard, an endangered subspecies of African houbara endemic to the Canary Islands.

Methods

We analysed GPS locations of 43 houbaras in 2018–2021, using solar GSM/GPRS loggers provided with accelerometers. We assessed (1) the variation in their home range and core area with kernel density estimators in relation to several intrinsic and extrinsic factors and (2) their foraging habitat selection.

Results

Home ranges were smallest during the breeding season (November–April), when rains triggered a rapid growth of herbaceous vegetation. Displaying males and nesting females had smaller home ranges than individuals not involved in reproduction. Both sexes used almost exclusively non-cultivated land, selecting low density Launaea arborescens shrublands, pastures and green fallows as foraging habitats. Heavier males used smaller home ranges because they spent more time displaying at a fixed display site, while heavier females moved over larger areas during the mating period, probably visiting more candidate mates. During the non-breeding season (May–October), both sexes showed larger home ranges, shifting to high density shrubland, but also partly to cultivated land. They selected sweet potato fields, green fallows, alfalfas, orchards and irrigated fields, which offered highly valuable food resources during the driest months of the year.

Conclusions

Our study shows how Canarian houbara, originally a desert-dwelling species that uses mostly shrublands and pastures, has developed the necessary adaptations to benefit from resources provided by current low intensity farming practices in the study area. Maintaining appropriate habitat conditions in the eastern Canary islands should constitute a key conservation measure to prevent the extinction of this endangered houbara subspecies.

Background

The home ranges represents the space required by an animal to obtain resources necessary for survival and reproduction, and so defines the ecological requirements of a species [1,2,3]. Variation in home range is caused by several factors intrinsic and extrinsic to each population or species, such as density of individuals [4], habitat type and structure [5], body size [6], phase of the breeding cycle [7,8,9], and quantity and quality of food resources [10,11,12]. In a review of the functional relationships underlying the ecology of home range in birds, Rolando [13] found that food availability was the most important of eleven factors identified as relevant from the literature, and concluded that habitat selection was the most influential among four main processes determining home range size (i.e. habitat selection, breeding, mating, and flocking). Rolando’s [13] review highlights the importance of investigating which factors influence home range size, and also whether these same factors directly or indirectly affect foraging habitat selection patterns. These relationships between home range and foraging habitat selection are crucial to understand the ecology of animal species, and ultimately also their population structure and dynamics [14]. In order to establish habitat selection patterns that help us to better understand home range variation, it is necessary to know where individuals feed and what resources they use by studying fine-scale foraging locations, something that is only possible when behavioural data are available [15,16,17,18,19]. In a review about methods and most important questions in home range research, Fieberg [20] claimed that home range studies using behavioural data are very scarce, and recommended that more research should be done on this topic. In recent times, modern advances in tracking technologies have been increasingly used to study animal movement at high spatial and temporal resolution, helping us to understand the spatial ecology and habitat use of organisms [21,22,23,24,25]. However, few home range studies have benefited from these developments to relate foraging behaviour with habitat selection and home range use (see e.g., Zurell et al. [19]).

Finally, an aspect that may be decisive in determining home range and foraging habitat selection patterns in birds is whether the habitat is modified by agriculture. Over one third of all bird species use agricultural areas to some extent, particularly during the non-breeding season, and seeds, cultivated plants or weeds and animals associated to crops may represent a very relevant fraction of food resources for some species [26, 27]. It is therefore of high interest to determine to what extent the use of agricultural resources can influence the size of the home range and the selection of foraging habitat, particularly considering that croplands represent a large percentage of global land cover and are continuing to expand rapidly [28].

In this study, we used GSM/GPRS data loggers provided with GPS and accelerometers to investigate these relationships in Canarian houbara bustards (Chlamydotis undulata fuertaventurae), an endangered subspecies of African houbara endemic to the Canary Islands. This tracking technology allowed us to record very detailed data on individual home ranges, use of space, and foraging habitat selection. Previous studies had provided some data on home range and movements of houbara (C. u. undulata) and MacQueen’s bustards (C. macquenii) [29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37], but research on the endemic subspecies of the Canary Islands has only started very recently (Abril-Colón et al. [38]). This subspecies is restricted to the three easternmost islands of the Canarian archipelago: Lanzarote, Fuerteventura and La Graciosa. The population of Lanzarote, estimated at 440–452 individuals [39], represents the main stronghold, with about 80% of the total population [40,41,42]. This subspecies is classified as globally Vulnerable according to IUCN criteria [43], and a detailed knowledge of home range and foraging resources will help to understand how human pressures may affect its survival in an environment shared with millions of tourists visiting these small islands every year.

Our first objective was to analyze home range changes over the annual cycle in Canarian houbaras, in relation to the eight following intrinsic and extrinsic factors: (1) Sex. Home range may be expected to vary between sexes, given the sexual size dimorphism and sexual segregation in this species, the sexual differences in reproductive behaviour (territorial defense in males, chick rearing only by females), and different phenologies of territory occupation and migration [38]. (2) Season. Studies on home range seasonal variation are still scarce and have provided contradictory results, with some showing larger home ranges during the breeding season [44, 45] and others the opposite [46, 47]. In our case, we expected smaller home ranges during the breeding season in both sexes. (3) Reproductive status. Previous studies highlight its important effect on home range [48,49,50]. In the nominate subspecies of African houbara, females apparently visit various males to choose a mate and therefore need to move over a large area [32]. Thus, we expected females to have large home ranges during the mating phase, and small home ranges during the nesting period. In males, we expected displaying individuals to have smaller home ranges than non-displaying individuals. (4) Body weight and condition. Both have been found to influence home range in interspecific comparisons [2, 51, 52], and also within species [53]. In species where males defend territories, the physical condition is often related to territory size and quality [54, 55], with fitter males occupying better and often larger territories [56]. Thus, we hypothesized that houbaras with better body condition should have larger home ranges. (5) Body size. In many species, adult birds have longer wings than first-year or immature individuals [57,58,59,60,61,62]. In houbara bustards, as far as we know there are no data confirming a wing length increase with age, either in wild or in captive birds (Y. Hingrat, pers. com.), but first-year birds are known to usually retain outer primary feathers over the first year, and sometimes even up to an age of 24 months, and outer primaries are shorter and narrower in juvenile birds than in adults [63]. Therefore, it seems reasonable to assume that individuals with shortest wings in our sample were probably first- or second-year birds, and to expect that they should have larger home ranges during their juvenile-immature dispersal phase than adults.

Regarding extrinsic factors, we tested the following predicted relationships: (6) Density of individuals. In both sexes, we expected home range to be inversely related to population density due to density-dependence and competition with neighbours (e.g., [4, 53, 64, 65]. (7) Precipitation. Rainfall is strongly related with plant productivity. In arid environments, annual plants resume rapid growth after rainfall events [66] and plant productivity and diversity decrease after long-term reductions in water availability [67]. Therefore, we predicted that home range during dry months should be substantially larger than in rainy months. (8) Habitat quality. Many species have been found to use smaller home ranges in more productive areas [12, 68,69,70], or in areas with higher food availability [71,72,73,74], so we predicted that home range would be inversely related to plant productivity.

Our second objective was to investigate the use houbaras make of available food resources. Like in home range, we expected that habitat use should also differ between sexes, reproductive status and seasons, particularly in a semi-desert environment where resources are scarce and may represent an important limiting factor. Finally, in the case of endangered species, as in our study, exploring these effects of environmental and behavioural factors simultaneously on home range and resource use helps to establish clear space use patterns that may serve as a basis for meaningful conservation and management plans [23, 75, 76].

Methods

Study area and species

The study was carried out in Lanzarote (Canary Islands, 29°02’, 13°37’W; 986 km2). The Canary archipelago is located in the Atlantic Ocean, 140 km west of the northwestern coast of Africa. The climate is subtropical-desert, tempered by the cold Canary Current and the permanent northeasterly “Trade” winds. The rainfall is concentrated in December-February, with an average 110 mm per year. Summers are dry, with less than 1 mm precipitation in June–August. The island has a volcanic origin, and the vegetation is characterized by xerophytic shrubs, modified in some areas by goat grazing and farming activities. The mosaic of uncultivated and cultivated land in the centre of the island, with a combination of shrublands, fallows and sweet potato fields, most of them irrigated, facilitates weed growth and attracts houbara bustards in summer [38].

African houbara bustards are polygynous [77,78,79], and exhibit an exploded lek mating system [80]. They show a moderate male-biased sexual size dimorphism [81], own unpublished data for the Canarian subspecies). The endemic subspecies C.u. fuertaventurae is a nocturnal and partial migrant. Over one third of the individuals abandon their breeding areas and migrate to non-breeding areas with a mosaic of shrubland and cropland where they spend the hottest and driest months of the year [38]. Between late autumn and early spring, males of both, the nominate C. u. undulata and the insular subspecies concentrate at their lek areas where males display at specific locations of their territories, to which they generally remain faithful over the whole breeding period and also between years [38]. Females visit displaying males for mating and take over all breeding duties [82]. Successfully breeding females normally raise one, less frequently two or three chicks that remain dependent of her for several months. Females with dependent chicks abandon their breeding area later than unsuccessful females [38].

Monitoring marked birds

Between 2017 and 2019, 43 houbaras (22 males and 21 females) were captured using nylon snares at display (males) or feeding sites (females). All birds were equipped with backpack-mounted, solar GSM/GPRS loggers (48 g model for males, 25 g model for females; e-obs GmbH, Gruenwald, Germany), using a soft, elastic band as harness material. The weight of transmitter plus harness was on average 2.83% of the body weight in males (range 2.54–3.15) and 2.15% in females (range 1.81–2.53). Males were captured at their display sites, which were selected randomly over the whole island, and females at foraging areas during the non-breeding season, in order not to jeopardize their nesting process. The capture team consisted of four people, who remained at 300–500 m from the capture site to be ready for access as soon as birds got entangled in the snares. Captured birds were immobilized, and their heads covered during the marking process to minimize capture stress. The average processing time of an individual from capture to release was 14 min (range 5–25). We did not observe behavioural alterations of the birds as a result of marking. Sex was established in the field using distinctive plumage features of females and males [83, 84], and confirmed by genetic analysis using DNA extracted from 1–2 contour feathers plucked from each bird.

The loggers recorded GPS locations between 05:00 and 22:59 UTC, i.e. from 1 (summer solstice) to 3 h (winter solstice) before sunrise to 1 to 3 h after sunset. All loggers were provided with an accelerometer (ACC) that registered the acceleration of the bird on a three-dimensional space, providing a 3D-graph representation of the bird's movements. We programmed the ACC between 05:00 and 21:00 UTC, obtaining 15-s activity bouts every 15 min, with a byte count of 1188 and 16.7 Hz. During the breeding season, we programmed the ACC of males during only 30 min (07:30–08:00 UTC), but using an intensive recording schedule of 45-s activity bouts every minute, because we were interested in recording in detail their display behaviour during the period of most intensive display at dawn for other studies (see more details below, in section Habitat use and selection analyses), and ACCs don't allow double programming within a day. The loggers were programmed to record one GPS location every five minutes when the charge level was high (95% of the time) and every 30 min otherwise. Of all ACC data recorded, we selected those coinciding with a GPS location, i.e. every 5 min, in order to be able to relate location with activity. Data were collected and stored in the Movebank repository (https://www.movebank.org/), and downloaded through the phone network, without having to recapture the birds.

Data processing

A total of 3.2 million GPS locations from two years (2018, 2019) were used in this study. We distinguished two seasons in the annual cycle of houbaras, breeding and non-breeding [38]. Due to differences in body size and weight, ecological requirements, territory occupation phenology and migration dates of males and females (Abril-Colón et al. [38]; own unpubl. data), and because in the nominate subspecies both sexes exhibit different space and habitat use patterns [32, 80], we decided to analyse home range and resource use of each sex separately. In males, we defined as Breeding season the period they spent in their territories defending a display site. This period was delimited by the first and last displays (as a rule, respectively, early November–December and March–April). In females, the breeding season started with their first visit to the nesting area and finished on the last day of their nesting attempt (respectively, late November and April) [38]. In males that did not perform sexual display, we set the start and end of their breeding season on the average starting and ending dates of the display period of all males displaying on that year. Similarly, in non-nesting females we established the start of their breeding season on the average date when nesting females visited their nesting areas, and the end when all females breeding successfully had finished rearing their chicks. In both sexes, the rest of the year was considered Non-breeding season.

We analysed the effect of factors intrinsic and extrinsic to the individual on their home range size. As intrinsic factors we considered body size, body weight and reproductive status. For each bird we established a Reproductive Status based on the classification of Hingrat et al. [80]. During the breeding season we distinguished two reproductive statuses in males: 1) Displaying, when the male showed some sexual activity (vocalization, display run or pre-copulatory movements, as revealed by accelerometry), and 2) Not displaying, when the male didn't show any sexual activity over the whole breeding season, either because it was immature, or due to a suboptimal body condition on that particular year. In females, we distinguished four reproductive statuses: 1) Mating, when the female visited males during the mating period; 2) Nesting, when the female was incubating (incubation lasts about 23 days; [82, 85], (3) Brooding, when the female had 1–2 months-old dependent chicks; 4) Not breeding, when the female did not breed on that year. During the rest of the annual cycle (non-breeding season), all individuals were qualified with a Non-reproductive status. We used Body weight as an indicator of fat content and general body condition, instead of the more commonly used index of weight/tarsus length, because the tarsus was not measured in all individuals in order to minimise handling time [86]. As an index of Body size we used the wing length [60, 62, 87]. In many bird species, and specifically in those more related to houbaras like other bustards and cranes, larger males are usually older and/or dominant [58, 59, 88,89,90,91], thus wing length may be also a proxy for age and dominance status.

We also analysed the following extrinsic factors: Precipitation was obtained from the nearest of a total of seven meteorological stations available in Lanzarote island, located at an average distance of 2.13 ± 1.42 km. We obtained the Density of individuals from a census carried out in the breeding season of 2018 (census dates: 19 January-23 February; [39]. As for the non-breeding season, in order to minimize errors due to the higher mobility and lower faithfulness to a specific site during that period [38], we used the average values of two surveys, respectively 20 May–21 June 2018 and 22 May–11 June 2019. In order to obtain more representative density estimates, these were not restricted to just the area within the home range of our marked individuals,instead, we calculated them separately for eight regions: Zonzamas, Tahiche, Playa Blanca, Teseguite, Teguise, Costa Teguise, Playa Quemada and Soo. All houbara surveys were done by two teams each of two people driving vehicles at low speed through tracks and roads (more details in [39]. As a proxy of Habitat quality, we used the Soil-Adjusted Vegetation Index (SAVI), a vegetation index developed as a modification of the Normalized Difference Vegetation Index (NDVI) to correct for the influence of soil brightness when vegetative cover is low [92, 93]. SAVI is a more suitable indicator than NDVI in arid and semiarid environments like Lanzarote [94]. Autocorrelation is often cited as a problem associated with kernel density and other home range estimators (e.g., [95,96,97,98]. However, several authors have argued against sub-sampling of data [99, 100] and defend that the autocorrelation is not an issue for home range estimation, but has largely been a red herring, drawing attention away from the more important issue of obtaining a representative sample of locations [99, 101,102,103]. In studies based on telemetry, large samples are always better because they are more representative [102], and references therein).

Home range analyses

Home range sizes were estimated using kernel density estimators (KDE) [104] and minimum convex polygons (MCP) [105, 106]. Kernel methods are the most statistically efficient nonparametric density estimators [107], since they do not make assumptions about the data distribution [108,109,110,111]. We used 50% kernels (KDE50) to delimit the core or most used areas, 95% kernels (KDE95) to define the total home range area, and MCP98 to represent the maximum area used by individuals, which was calculated including all outlier locations. We used the “reproducible home ranges” package (rhr) in R software for statistical computing [112, 113]. We established the reference bandwidth “href” as smoothing parameter for all individual home range estimations (see e.g., [114,115,116]). Although “href” may include areas outside an individual´s home range and thus may be positively biased, it shows a closer match between estimated and true home ranges with increasing sample size, so it is particularly recommended when sample size is large, as in our case [115]. We used monthly kernels for each bird as data points in the analyses, ignoring the tagging day and the two subsequent days to exclude any possible anomalous behaviour due to the capture and marking process. Periods of more than 15 days were considered as whole months, and those of less than 15 days were discarded. For each individual we used data from an average tracking period of 12.7 months (SD = 6.66, range 3–24). We also calculated MCPs, but did not use them in subsequent generalized linear mixed models (GLMMs). MCPs clearly overestimate true home ranges due to their lack of concavity and assumption of equal use of all locations [96].

We used the Wilcoxon signed-rank test to examine differences in home range size between seasons, and the Mann-Whitney test to examine differences between sexes and reproductive statuses, and Chi-squared test to examine foraging habitat selection. For each sex, we used GLMMs [117] to test variation in home range size over the annual cycle with the factors as described above: (1) season, (2) breeding status, (3) precipitation, (4) density of females, (5) density of males, (6) body size, (7) body weight, (8) habitat quality and (9) the interaction between season and precipitation. We considered this interaction because the effects of season on home range size could be expected to vary under different precipitation regimes, a frequent phenomenon in a semiarid area like the eastern Canary Islands. For example, we have detected a relationship between the onset of breeding and precipitation (pers. obs.). Since we had repeated measures of the same birds in two years, we included individual and year as random factors in GLMMs to avoid pseudoreplication. The response variable showed a negative binomial distribution and thus models were run using a loglink function. We used model averaging to calculate the predicted values. After computing parameter estimates averaged over all models of the dataset, we weighed them by using Akaike criterion at each model [118]. For all candidate models, we calculated the relevant parameters including the Akaike information criterion (AIC). Differences in AIC compared to the lowest AIC (ΔAIC), Akaike weight (Wi), deviance explained and degrees of freedom were calculated, and the best model based on AIC was identified [119]. We obtained the parameter estimates and confidence intervals for each covariate included in all candidate models with > 5% of the weight of evidence (Additional file 1: Tables S2, S3). GLMMs were performed using the function glmer of the “lme4” package [120]. GLMMs were also performed to analyse variation in core areas. Since results were identical to those of home range analyses (see uploaded Related file), here we present only GLMM results for home range. The variables used did not show any collinearity problems. Mann-Whitney and Wilcoxon signed-rank tests were performed using the “wilcox.test function” and Chi-squared test with the “chisq.test function” in software R version 3.6.3 (https://www.r-project.org).

Habitat use and selection analyses

In addition to exploring which variables explained the size of home ranges, we investigated which habitat types houbaras selected for foraging within their home range. We could carry out these analyses thanks to accelerometers (ACC), which enabled us to identify the ACC graph patterns and sequences that corresponded to the main activities of houbaras. To learn which graph pattern corresponded to each behaviour, we observed 10 marked birds from December 2018 to March 2019 (720 h in total) using a 20–60 × telescope from a distance of ca.1000 m. We recorded the timing of each behaviour and compared these timed field observations with the ACC graphs. We used the sequences of these behaviours to train a model using Accelerater, a software based on supervised machine learning, and translated ACC records to behavioural modes [121, 122], http://smell.huji.ac.il/). The model was trained on 2555 ground-truthed ACC sequences of known behaviours, and it classified ten behavioral modes: display run, pre-copulatory movement, vocalization, flying, foraging, laying down, pre-display posture, preening, running and vigilant posture. After assigning each GPS location to one of these behaviours identified from the ACC graphs, we filtered the subsample of foraging locations to analyze resource selection. Among various model types available in Accelerater, we obtained RBF SVM as the best one, with 92.95% correct classification (SD = 0.67), which means that all behaviours were identified with more than 90% accuracy.

As explained above, during the breeding season the ACCs of males were programmed to record acceleration data only from 07:30 to 08:00 h, so for breeding males we knew their behaviour only during that half-an-hour period. In order to estimate to what extent this could affect the foraging habitat use and selection results, we performed all calculations using both, this 30 min-ACC sample and the sample of GPS locations covering the period of maximum foraging activity (08:30 to 11:00 h), for which we had a GPS location every 5 min, but no behaviour associated to each GPS location. In other words, for that 2.5 h period we knew where each male was located, but not whether it was foraging, walking, or displaying. We did not appreciate any differences between both samples (07:30–08:00 h, and 08:30–11:00 h) and therefore we decided to use the 07:30–08:00 h locations in all subsequent analyses.

To analyse foraging habitat selection, we compared the GPS locations where each marked bird was feeding with a similar number of random points (pseudo-absences) generated within its home range. To prevent these pseudo-absences being on the same fields as foraging locations, we established a buffer of twenty metres for each foraging location and ensured that no pseudo-absence fell within those buffers. Since unbalanced prevalences may provide unstable and unreliable estimates of discriminatory power [123], a sampling effort was made to arrive at a balanced prevalence between presence (foraging locations) and pseudo-absence data (random locations). To increase the sample of breeding females and make it more similar to that of displaying males, in addition to the 6 females that bred in 2018–2019 we included breeding location data from 9 females in 2021.

We obtained the farming status and characteristics of all fields within the home range of each houbara from the 2020 Crop Production Map for Lanzarote (https://www.gobiernodecanarias.org/agricultura/temas/mapa_cultivos/lanzarote/), and completed or modified it with the 2018 orthophoto (http://www.grafcan.es) and direct observations of each field on the ground. This ground truthing was done by visiting all fields comprised within the home ranges of our marked birds and talking to as many owners as possible to determine the farming cycle phenology and other details of each field. The habitat types considered in this study were: Pastures; Shrubland of Launaea arborescens, where we distinguished two shrub density categories, high and low; Fallow, distinguishing a) Sweet potato fallow (where sweet potato was grown in the previous year), b) White fallow (ploughed continuously to prevent weed growth), c) Green fallow (no longer cultivated, weeds are left to grow); Alfalfa Medicago sativa fields, Orchards, Clean orchards (orchards where weeds are not allowed to grow), Sweet potato fields, and Sweet potato/fallow (mostly sweet potato crops with annual rotation of a cultivated sector and an uncultivated sector in the same field).

We compared the distributions of foraging and pseudo-absence locations of both sexes during the two seasons, breeding and non-breeding, using separate GLMMs for each sex and season. In order to get well-defined resource selection patterns for these two seasons, we used data from only the central months of the season when all birds of the sample were either breeding or had already started a post-breeding phase, i.e. December to February for the breeding season, and June to August for the non-breeding season. To avoid pseudoreplication we used data from only one year for each bird. We used GLMMs with binomial error structure to infer resource selection from use-availability data with presence vs pseudo-absence as response variable, individual as random factor, and habitat types (pastures, high or low density shrubland, sweet potato fallow, white fallow, green fallow, alfalfa, orchards, clean orchards, sweet potato and sweet potato/fallow-fields) as predictors. Presences and pseudo-absences for the resource selection analysis were plotted in ArcGIS, and software R version 3.6.3 (https://www.r-project.org) was used for all statistical analyses using the packages rhr [113], MuMIn [124] and lme4 [120].

Results

Home range, core area and maximum area used

During the breeding season, the 19 displaying males and 6 breeding females used smaller home ranges (K95), core areas (K50), and maximum areas (MCP98) than the 6 males that did not display, and 18 females that did not breed (males: respectively, W = 13, p = 0.005; W = 13, p = 0.003; W = 15, p = 0.006; females: W = 14, p = 0.013; W = 13, p = 0.009; W = 14, p = 0.013; Mann-Whitney test; Table 1; see monthly values for both sexes in Additional file 1: Table S1). During the non-breeding season, the 19 males that had displayed used larger home ranges than during the breeding season (V = 16, p = 0.002 in K95; V = 8.5, p < 0.001 in K50; V = 29, p = 0.008 in MCP98; Wilcoxon signed-rank test). The same trend was observed in the 6 females that had bred, although female differences did not reach significance, probably due to the small sample size (V = 3, p = 0.625 in K95; V = 1, p = 0.250 in K50; V = 4, p = 0.875 in MCP98; Wilcoxon signed-rank test). During the non-breeding season, the home ranges of the 19 males that had displayed and those of the 6 females that had bred did not significantly differ in size from the home ranges used during the breeding season by the 6 males that did not display (W = 44.5, p = 0.859 in K95, W = 63, p = 0.862 in K50: W = 46.5, p = 0.524 in MCP98) and the 18 females that did not breed (W = 24, p = 0.721 in K95, W = 27, p = 0.487 in K50 and W = 27, p = 0.957 MCP98; Mann-Whitney test; Table 1).

Table 1 Mean home-range (KD95), core area (KD50) and maximum area used (MCP98) by male and female houbara bustards depending on reproductive status and season

As for sexual differences, during the breeding season there were no differences in home range size between displaying males and breeding females (W = 68, p = 0.224 in K95; W = 60, p = 0.113 in K50; W = 79.5, p = 0.491 in MCP98), nor between non-displaying males and non-breeding females (W = 32.5, p = 0.555 in K95, W = 26, p = 0.953 in K50, W = 37, p = 0.2625 in MCP98; Mann-Whitney test; Table 1). During the non-breeding season, there were no sexual differences in home range size (W = 213, p = 0.885 in K95; W = 183, p = 0.693 in K50; W = 181.5, p = 0.480 MCP98; Mann–Whitney test; Table 1). The mean overlap between post-breeding areas in different years was 61.6% in males and 47.2% in females. As for breeding areas, the mean overlap was 52.1% in males and 42.2% in females (Additional file 1: Fig. S1).

Although the bivariate analysis did not show significant sexual differences within a given season, we performed generalized linear mixed models separately for males and females, because both sexes exhibit various contrasting features during their annual cycles (e.g. different territory occupation and migration phenologies, Abril-Colón et al. [38]), and certain factors could have distinct effects on male and female home ranges. Thus for each sex we obtained multiple models to describe home range (Tables 2, 3, details in Additional file 1: Tables S2, S3), of which for the sake of brevity and clarity here we make inferences only from those selected though Akaike criteria as the most plausible ones. The best model explaining home range in males included season, precipitation, reproductive status, body weight and habitat quality, plus the interaction between season and precipitation (Table 2). A second valid model included also the effect of body size. Sums of the AIC weights showed season, reproductive status, precipitation and body weight to be the best predictors of home-range size (wi = 1). These models confirmed the variation of home range with season and with reproductive status, with non-displaying individuals using larger areas than displaying individuals (Additional file 1: Table S2). In addition, home ranges were larger when precipitation, body weight and habitat quality were lower (Additional file 1: Table S2). Also, a positive interaction between season and precipitation was found.

Table 2 Candidate generalized linear mixed models analysing the effect of intrinsic and extrinsic factors on the home range size (K95) of males (n = 22)
Table 3 Candidate generalized linear mixed models analysing the effect of intrinsic and extrinsic factors on the home range size (K95) in female houbara bustards (n = 21)

As for home ranges of females, the first two plausible models showed almost identical weights, so both may be considered equally valid (Table 3). These models included the effects of reproductive status, precipitation, density of males (first model only) or females (second model only), body size, body weight and habitat quality. A third, also valid model (ΔAIC < 2) included both, density of males and density of females. The sums of AIC weights showed reproductive status, precipitation, habitat quality, body size, body weight, density of males and density of females to be the best predictors of home range size (wi > 0.656). As in males, reproductive status had in females a significant and even higher effect than in males, with nesting females showing the smallest areas, non-breeding females the largest, and mating and brooding intermediate values (Table 3, Additional file 1: Table S3, Additional file 1: Fig. S2). In females, home range increased with precipitation and habitat quality, in contrast to what happens in males. Female home range also increased when body size was smaller, and when density of females and males (first model and second model, Table 3) (were lower (Additional file 1: Table S3). Finally, the fourth and fifth candidate models included season, a variable that was not retained in the first three models (Table 3). These fourth and fifth models showed ΔAIC < 2 and therefore could also be considered plausible to some extent, but as shown in the bivariate analysis, the effect of season was apparently less pronounced in females than in males.

Selection of foraging habitat types

In both sexes, the distribution of foraging locations among habitat types differed from that of locations not used for foraging (males: X2 = 6568.68, df = 10, p < 0.001 in the breeding season; X2 = 27,179.15, df = 10, p < 0.001 in the non-breeding season; females: X2 = 1158.17, df = 10, p < 0.001 in breeding season, X2 = 4137.94, df = 10, p < 0.001 in the non-breeding season; Chi-square test). During the breeding season, displaying males and breeding females foraged almost exclusively on non-cultivated land, using primarily low-density Launaea shrubland and pastures (Additional file 1: Table S4). The only two non-displaying males and four females of our sample that did not breed used pastures as their main foraging habitat, followed in the case of females by sweet potato, green and sweet potato fallows, and low-density shrubland (Additional file 1: Table S4).

During the non-breeding season, the use of cultivated land increased in both sexes, reaching 26.58% of all foraging locations in females, and 17.73% in males. This seasonal increase in the use of cultivated fields was significant in both sexes (X2 = 1069.2, df = 1, p < 0.001 in males, X2 = 4626.9, df = 1, p < 0.001 in females; Chi-square test; Additional file 1: Table S4). A significant majority of these cultivated fields were irrigated (X2 = 1110, df = 1, p < 0.001, X2 = 365.3, df = 1, p < 0.001; Chi-square test, comparing locations in irrigated vs non-irrigated fields respectively in males and females; Additional file 1: Table S4). In this season both sexes continued showing a preference for Launaea shrubland as the main foraging ground, though males used higher density shrubland more than lower-density shrubland. Females spent more time foraging on sweet potato fields, green fallows and alfalfa fields than in the breeding season, and used these habitats more than males (Additional file 1: Table S4).

The results of GLMMs showed that houbara bustards chose foraging sites that were significantly different from randomly selected pseudo-absence locations. We found noteworthy seasonal differences and some sexual differences in foraging habitat selection. In the breeding season, displaying males selected low-density shrubland and green fallows as main foraging habitats, avoiding shrubland with high density coverage, sweet potato fallow and white fallows, while in the non-breeding season they showed a preference for high-density shrubland, sweet potato fields and orchards (Table 4; Additional file 1: Tables S5 and S6). As for females, during the breeding season they selected pastures, green and white fallows and sweet potato/fallow (Table 4; Additional file 1: Table S7). During the non-breeding season females showed similar selection patterns as males, avoiding pastures, white fallows, clean orchards and sweet potato/fallow, and selecting high-density shrubland and green fallows, together with sweet potato fields (Table 4; Additional file 1: Table S8).

Table 4 Estimates of generalized linear mixed models describing fine-scale foraging habitat of male and female houbara bustards during both seasons

Discussion

Our results show that much of the variation in home range and foraging habitat selection was determined by season and reproductive status. During the breeding season, displaying males and nesting females had much smaller home ranges than males and females not involved in reproduction. Among non-breeding individuals with large home ranges, our sample surely included some immature birds, as suggested by the negative relationship between home range and wing length found in models. This relationship was significant in females and also negative though not significant in males, in which the only size-related variable retained in the best model was body weight, also with negative effect on home range. These results agree with the large areas used by dispersing juveniles in the nominate houbara subspecies [125], as well as with the common pattern found in other bird species, where non-breeding adults, floaters, or dispersing immatures show larger home ranges than breeding individuals [18, 19, 126, 127]; see review in [13].

The small home ranges found in displaying males and the largest home ranges of mating females are consistent with the exploded lek mating system attributed to this species [31, 32, 37, 38, 80]. Within the lek area, male houbaras defend small territories where they display over several months [128] They remain faithful to these territories throughout the whole breeding season and between years [38, 129]. As for breeding females, their home ranges are large during the mating phase, when they typically visit several males [32] and even mate with more than one as suggested by the high proportion of broods with multipaternity [130]. The positive effect of body weight on home range found only in females suggests that females moving over larger areas and visiting more males are those in better condition. In males, home range is negatively related to body weight, because males in better condition spend more time displaying and therefore move less during that period than lighter, non-displaying males. Individual and seasonal variation in weight is higher in males than in females ([131,132,133], own unpubl. data for the Canarian subspecies). Males gain 15–20% weight to reach highest weights at the start of the breeding season, losing weight considerably during the display period when they spend little time feeding [37, 128]). Heavier males display more intensively and weight loss during display is correlated with display intensity, so weight represents a signal of male quality [131, 134,135,136], explaining the negative relationship between weight and home range found in our models. Differences related to reproductive status disappeared during the non-breeding season, when home ranges of individuals that had been involved in breeding activities equaled those of non-breeding and immature individuals, and all birds showed larger home ranges than during the breeding season.

Precipitation and habitat quality also showed opposite effects in males and females. Although monthly values of these two variables were not highly correlated and thus we included both in home range models, to some extent they have similar meanings in relation to reproduction in houbara bustards. Both had a positive effect on home range in female models and a negative effect in male models. Precipitation has a strong influence on circannual rhythms of species inhabiting arid environments [137,138,139,140,141], and these effects have been also documented in houbara bustards. For example, breeding is suppressed in dry years, and rainfall seems to trigger the onset of breeding in this species ([142,143,144], own unpubl. data), possibly through mechanisms inducing gonadal activity [133]. As for SAVI, it has been commonly used as a proxy for net primary production in poorly vegetated environments such as our study area [94]. As expected, monthly values of both precipitation and habitat quality had similar effects on the breeding phenology of houbaras because autumn and winter rainfalls produce a rapid growth of herbaceous vegetation, inducing males to start displaying and females to begin searching for a mate. This happens in less than a month, explaining why both variables produce the same effect within each sex but opposite effects on male and female monthly home range models. Shortly after the first rainfalls males occupy their territories and reduce their home range to a minimum, whereas females increase it while searching for mates.

With respect to differences in foraging habitat related to season and reproductive status, we found that during the breeding season both sexes showed a preference either for low density shrubland or pastures, though the most selected of these habitats differed between sexes, males preferring shrubland and females pasture land. This sexual difference might only be due to the fact that males use high points to display, and these are usually occupied by shrubland, whereas pastures are mostly on valleys. In any case, both habitat types really differ from each other only very slightly, representing quite areas where both sexes can carry out their breeding activities without being disturbed. Both offer similar amounts and diversities of weeds and invertebrates consumed by breeding houbaras [145, 146]. A third, much less used but still selected foraging habitat were green fallows, where weeds are allowed to grow and thus offer similar food as pastures or shrublands. During this season, both sexes avoided foraging in high density shrubland, probably because in this habitat visibility is hindered by bushes. A good visibility is crucial for displaying males [37, 129, 147] and for nesting females of the nominate subspecies, which avoid areas with tall vegetation [37]. In great bustards, males and females also select display and nesting sites with good visibility, probably to attract more females in the case of males, and to reduce predation pressure in both sexes [148, 149]. In houbaras, concealment provided while foraging by low density shrubland might be appropriate for nesting and brooding females, but a too high shrub density might not be appropriate.

During the non-breeding season foraging habitat models showed a shift to high density shrubland where birds find quiet and hidden sites for feeding in summer, no significant selection for low density shrubland, and negative selection for pastures. During this season, birds of both sexes increased significantly the use of cultivated land, particularly sweet potato fields, and selected irrigated over non-irrigated fields. A previous study found that in the island of Fuerteventura houbaras selected gavias as foraging grounds, a traditional mode of cultivation designed to retain rain and runoff water equivalent to modern irrigation systems [146]. Although natural grasslands are originally the main habitat of the family Otididae [81], several species forage on farmland, and increase the use of cultivated fields during the non-breeding season, e.g. the Great Bustard (Otis tarda) ([150,151,152,153,154]), the Little Bustard (Tetrax tetrax) [155, 156], or the MacQueeen's bustard [157]. Cultivated areas represent an additional food resource for many other birds, which have adapted to human-induced landscape changes, and in some cases have become agriculture specialists [158, 159]).

Regarding sexual variation, in contrast to what we expected, we found no differences between males and females in their average home ranges during either the breeding or non-breeding season. Neither we found important sexual differences in foraging habitat selection. Essentially, both sexes had the same requirements regarding home range and food resources. It seems that sexual dimorphism, sexual segregation, and differences in roles during reproduction and in phenologies of migration and display and nesting territory occupation are not enough to cause great sexual differences in average home range size or foraging habitat, either in the breeding or non-breeding season. However, sexual differences in behaviour could have indeed determined the different sign of the relationship between home range and some of the variables in our models. For example, an increasing density of conspecifics didn't seem to affect much the home range of males, whereas it determined a significant reduction of home range in females. A density-dependent restriction on home range size is expected when breeding habitat is limited [12, 64, 160,161,162,163]. Density-dependent effects on home range may probably be small during the non-breeding season, when houbaras can aggregate in small flocks of up to 5–6 individuals, but may affect territories of nesting and brooding females due to competition with neighbour females or sexual harassment when male density is high. The size of male territories, in contrast, didn't seem to be as density-dependent as that of females. A male territory may be restricted to the minimum area necessary to perform display and mating activities without being disturbed by neighbour males, and thus probably does not extend farther even under low conspecific densities, simply because a larger territory is not economically defendable. In support of this conclusion, the variability and maximum extent of home ranges were smaller in displaying males than in breeding females. In sum, home range sizes of males and females seem to fit with the exploded lek mating system attributed to houbara bustards. While in classical lek species female home ranges are larger than those of males, in resource-defence polygyny systems both sexes show no differences in home range size, and exploded leks occupy an intermediate position in this gradient [164,165,166].

Beyond the small sexual differences in density-dependence discussed above, home ranges and core areas of Canarian houbaras are much smaller than those reported for the nominate subspecies in north Africa (home ranges of 17 km2 in males and 146 km2 in females, core areas of about 1 km2; [32], and for MacQueen’s bustard (home ranges of 116–977 km2, core areas of 13–128 km2 [142, 151].

Conclusions

Male and female breeding home ranges were small during the breeding season, allowing for a high density of houbaras in the study area. During the breeding season both sexes selected non-cultivated habitats with low shrub coverage as foraging grounds, avoiding high density shrubland. During the non-breeding season Launaea arborescens shrubland continued to be the main foraging habitat, but both sexes spent a significant amount of time foraging on cultivated fields, and notably selected irrigated farmland (18% foraging locations in males, 27% in females). These irrigated fields seem to be of considerable value for the survival of houbaras during the driest months of the year, when natural resources in shrublands and pastures are scarcer. However, these cultivated fields need to be close enough to high density shrubland areas, the major foraging ground during that season, in order to be accessible to houbaras during their daily foraging routines. Thus, an appropriate mixture of shrubland, green fallows and a few irrigated fields seem to be the best habitat for houbaras in that season.

Finally, our study shows how Canarian houbaras, originally a desert-dwelling species, have developed the necessary adaptations to benefit from resources provided by current farmland conditions in our study area. Maintaining current habitat conditions in Lanzarote, i.e. a mosaic of Launaea shrubland with adequate amounts of fallows and irrigated cultivated fields, and improving those in Fuerteventura, should be prioritized as management measures in order to safeguard the future of this endangered species.

Availability of data and materials

The data that support the findings of this study are available from Red Eléctrica de España. Restrictions apply to the availability of these data, which were used under license for this study. Data are also available from the authors with the permission from Red Eléctrica de España.

Abbreviations

GLMMs:

Generalized linear mixed models

KDE:

Kernel density estimator

NDVI:

Normalized difference vegetation index

SAVI:

Soil-adjusted vegetation index

References

  1. Burt WH. Territoriality and home range concepts as applied to marnmals. J Mammal. 1943;24(3):346–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. McNab BK. Bioenergetics and the determination of home range size. Am Nat. 1963;97(894):133–40. https://doi.org/10.1086/282264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Reiss M. Scaling of home range size: body size, metabolic needs and ecology. Trends Ecol Evol. 1988;3(3):85–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/0169-5347(88)90025-0.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Morse DH. Variables affecting the density and territory size of breeding spruce-woods warblers. Ecology. 1976;57(2):290–301. https://doi.org/10.2307/1934817.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Diemer KM, Nocera JJ. Associations of bobolink territory size with habitat quality. Ann Zool Fennici. 2014;51(6):515–25. https://doi.org/10.5735/086.051.0607.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Mazerolle DF, Hobson KA. Territory size and overlap in male Ovenbirds: contrasting a fragmented and contiguous boreal forest. Can J Zool. 2004;82(11):1774–81. https://doi.org/10.1139/z04-175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Brothers N, Gales R, Hedd A, Robertson G. Foraging movements of the shy albatross Diomedea cauta breeding in Australia; implications for interactions with longline fisheries. Ibis. 1998;140:446–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Mazur KM, Frith SD, James PC. Barred owl home range and habitat selection in the Boreal forest of central Saskatchewan. Auk. 1998;115:746–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Poonswad P, Tsun A. Ranges of males of the Great Hornbill Buceros bicomis, Brown Hornbill Ptilolaemus tickelli and Wreathed Hornbill Rhyticeros undulatus in Khao Yai National Park, Thailand. Ibis. 1994;136:79–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Börger L, Dalziel BD, Fryxell JM. Are there general mechanisms of animal home range behaviour? A review and prospects for future research: home range modelling. Ecol Lett. 2008;11(6):637–50. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1461-0248.2008.01182.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Buchmann CM, Schurr FM, Nathan R, Jeltsch F. An allometric model of home range formation explains the structuring of animal communities exploiting heterogeneous resources. Oikos. 2011;120(1):106–18. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0706.2010.18556.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. McLoughlin PD, Ferguson SH. A hierarchical pattern of limiting factors helps explain variation in home range size. Ecoscience. 2000;7:123130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Rolando A. On the ecology of home range in birds. Revue d’Ecologie Terre et Vie. 2002;57(1):53–73.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Lowe WH, McPeek MA. Is dispersal neutral? Trends Ecol Evol. 2014;29(8):444–50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2014.05.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Kaczensky P, Adiya Y, von Wehrden H, Mijiddorj B, Walzer C, Güthlin D, et al. Space and habitat use by wild Bactrian camels in the Transaltai Gobi of southern Mongolia. Biol Conserv. 2014;169(100):311–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biocon.2013.11.033.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Marzluff JM, Knick ST, Millspaugh JJ. High-tech behavioral ecology: modeling the distribution of animal activities to better understand wildlife space use and resource selection. In: Millspaugh JJ, Marzluff KM, editors. Radio Tracking and Animal Populations. Academic Press. Elsevier; San Diego, California. 2001. p. 309–26.

  17. Silva-Opps M, Opps SB. Use of telemetry data to investigate home range and habitat selection in mammalian carnivores. Modern Telemetry. Ondrej K, editor. InTech Open; 2011. p. 281–306.

  18. Tanferna A, López-Jiménez L, Blas J, Hiraldo F, Sergio F. Habitat selection by Black kite breeders and floaters: implications for conservation management of raptor floaters. Biol Conserv. 2013;160:1–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biocon.2012.12.031.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Zurell D, von Wehrden H, Rotics S, Kaatz M, Groß H, Schlag L, et al. Home range size and resource use of breeding and non-breeding white storks along a land use gradient. Front Ecol Evol. 2018. https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2018.00079.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Fieberg J, Börger L. Could you please phrase “home range” as a question? J Mammal. 2012;93(4):890–902. https://doi.org/10.1644/11-mamm-s-172.1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Bograd SJ, Block BA, Costa DP, Godley BJ. Biologging technologies: new tools for conservation. Introduction. Endanger Species Res. 2010;10:1–7. https://doi.org/10.3354/esr00269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Cooke SJ, Hinch SG, Wikelski M, Andrews RD, Kuchel LJ, Wolcott TG, et al. Biotelemetry: a mechanistic approach to ecology. Trends Ecol Evol. 2004;19(6):334–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2004.04.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Kays R, Crofoot MC, Jetz W, Wikelski M. Terrestrial animal tracking as an eye on life and planet. Science. 2015;348(6240):aaa2478. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aaa2478.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. López-López P. Individual-based tracking systems in ornithology: Welcome to the era of big data. Ardeola. 2016;63(1):103. https://doi.org/10.13157/arla.63.1.2016.rp5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Rutz C, Hays GC. New frontiers in biologging science. Biol Lett. 2009;5(3):289–92. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2009.0089.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Blount JD, Horns JJ, Kittelberger KD, Neate-Clegg MH, Şekercioğlu ÇH. Avian use of agricultural areas as migration stopover sites: a review of crop management practices and ecological correlates. Front Ecol Evol. 2021;2021(9):650641.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Şekercioğlu ÇH. Conservation ecology: area trumps mobility in fragment bird extinctions. Curr Biol. 2007;17:283–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.02.018.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. Ramankutty N, Foley JA. Estimating historical changes in global land cover: croplands historical have converted areas. Glob Biogeochem Cycles. 1999;13:997–1027. https://doi.org/10.1029/1999GB900046.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Burnside RJ, Collar NJ, Dolman PM. Comparative migration strategies of wild and captive-bred Asian Houbara Chlamydotis macqueenii. Ibis. 2017;159(2):374–89. https://doi.org/10.1111/ibi.12462.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Burnside RJ, Buchan C, Salliss D, Collar NJ, Dolman PM. Releases of Asian houbara must respect genetic and geographic origin to preserve inherited migration behaviour: evidence from a translocation experiment. R Soc Open Sci. 2020;7(3):200250. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.200250.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Hingrat Y, Saint JM. Mating system of the Houbara Bustard Chlamydotis undulata undulata in eastern Morocco. Ardeola. 2005;52(1):91–102.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Hingrat Y, Jalme MS, Ysnel F, Lacroix F, Seabury J, Rautureau P. Relationships between home-range size, sex and season with reference to the mating system of the Houbara Bustard Chlamydotis undulata undulata: Mating system of the Houbara Bustard. Ibis. 2004;146(2):314–22. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1474-919x.2004.00263.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Judas J, Combreau O, Lawrence M, Saleh M, Launay F, Xingyi G. Migration and range use of Asian Houbara Bustard Chlamydotis macqueenii breeding in the Gobi Desert, China, revealed by satellite tracking: Migration and range use of Asian Houbara Bustard. Ibis (Lond 1859). 2006;148(2):343–51. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1474-919x.2006.00546.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Mansouri J. Status of Houbara Bustard Chlamydotis undulata in five important habitats in Iran. Iran J Ornithol. 2006;1:17–20.

    Google Scholar 

  35. Riou S, Combreau O, Judas J, Lawrence M, Al Baidani MS, Pitra C. Genetic differentiation among migrant and resident populations of the threatened Asian houbara bustard. J Hered. 2012;103(1):64–70. https://doi.org/10.1093/jhered/esr113.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Tourenq C, Combreau O, Lawrence M, Launay F. Migration patterns of four Asian Houbara Chlamydotis macqueenii wintering in the Cholistan Desert, Punjab, Pakistan. Bird Conserv Int. 2004. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0959270904000012.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Hingrat Y, Saint Jalme M, Chalah T, Orhant N, Lacroix F. Environmental and social constraints on breeding site selection. Does the exploded-lek and hotspot model apply to the Houbara bustard Chlamydotis undulata undulata? J Avian Biol. 2008;39(4):393–404. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0908-8857.2008.03994.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Abril-Colón I, Alonso JC, Palacín C, Álvarez-Martínez JM, Ucero A. Short distance nocturnal migration in an island endemic bustard. Ibis. 2022;164:1145–59. https://doi.org/10.1111/ibi.13061.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Alonso JC, Palacín C, Abril-Colón I. The Lanzarote Population of the African Houbara Chlamydotis undulata fuertaventurae: census, sex ratio, productivity, and a proposed new survey method. Ardeola. 2020;67(1):113–27.

    Google Scholar 

  40. Palacín C, Alonso JC. African Houbara (Chlamydotis undulata fuertaventurae). In Keller, V, Herrando S, Vorísek P, Franch M, Kipson M, Milanesi P, Martí D, Anton M, Klvanová A, Kalyakin M, Bauer HG, Foppen RPB, editors. European Breeding Bird Atlas 2: Distribution, Abundance and Change. Keller et al, editors. EBCC & Lynx Edicions, Barcelona. 2020; p. 233.

  41. Schuster C, Iglesias-Lebrija JJ, Carrascal LM. Recent population trends of the houbara bustard in the Canary Islands. Methods and conservation status. Anim Biodivers Conserv. 2012;35(1):125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Ucero A, Abril-Colón I, Palacín C, Alonso JC. Avutarda hubara canaria, Chlamydotis undulata fuertaventurae. In: López-Jiménez N, editor. Libro Rojo de las Aves de España. SEO/BirdLife. Madrid. 2021. p. 343–51.

  43. BirdLife International. Chlamydotis undulata. IUCN Red List for birds. 2021. Doi: https://doi.org/10.2305/IUCN.UK.2016-3.RLTS.T22728245A90341807.en.

  44. Kolts JR, McRae SB. Seasonal home range dynamics and sex diferences in habitat use in a threatened, coastal marsh bird. Ecol Evol. 2017;7:1101–11.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Pfeiffer T, Meyburg B-U. GPS tracking of Red Kites (Milvus milvus) reveals fledgling number is negatively correlated with home range size. J Ornithol. 2015;156(4):963–75. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10336-015-1230-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Morganti M, Assandri G, Aguirre JI, Ramirez Á, Caffi M, Pulido F. How residents behave: home range flexibility and dominance over migrants in a Mediterranean passerine. Anim Behav. 2017;123:293–304. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.10.021.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Wiktander U, Olsson O, Nilsson SG. Seasonal variation in home-range size, and habitat area requirement of the lesser spotted woodpecker (Dendrocopos minor) in southern Sweden. Biol Conserv. 2001;100(3):387–95. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0006-3207(01)00045-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Boydston EE, Kapheim KM, Szykman M, Holekamp KE. Individual variation in space use by female spotted hyenas. J Mammal. 2003;84(3):1006–18. https://doi.org/10.1644/bos-038.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Dahle B, Swenson JE. Home ranges in adult Scandinavian brown bears (Ursus arctos): effect of mass, sex, reproductive category, population density and habitat type. J Zool. 2003;260(4):329–35. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0952836903003753.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Fritzell EK. Habitat use by prairie raccoons during the waterfowl breeding season. J Wildl Manage. 1978;42(1):118. https://doi.org/10.2307/3800698.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Harestad AS, Bunnel FL. Home range and body weight–a reevaluation. Ecology. 1979;60(2):389–402. https://doi.org/10.2307/1937667.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Kelt DA, Van Vuren D. Energetic constraints and the relationship between body size and home range area in mammals. Ecology. 1999;80(1):337. https://doi.org/10.2307/177002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Schradin C. Territorial defence in a group-living solitary forager: who, where, against whom? Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2004;55:439–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Bart J, Earnst SL. Relative importance of male and territory quality in pairing success of male rock ptarmigan (Lagopus mutus). Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1999;45(5):355–9. https://doi.org/10.1007/s002650050571.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Price TD. Sexual selection on body size, territory and plumage variables in a population of Darwin’s finches. Evolution. 1984;38(2):327. https://doi.org/10.2307/2408491.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Candolin U, Voigt H-R. Correlation between male size and territory quality: consequence of male competition or predation susceptibility? Oikos. 2001;95(2):225–30. https://doi.org/10.1034/j.1600-0706.2001.950204.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Alatalo RV, Gustafsson L, Lundbkrg A. Why do young passerine birds have shorter wings than older birds? Ibis. 1984;126(3):410–5. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1474-919x.1984.tb00264.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Alonso JC, Magaña M, Palacín C, Martín CA. Correlates of male mating success in great bustard leks: the effects of age, weight, and display effort. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2010;64(10):1589–600. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-010-0972-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Alonso JC, Bautista LM, Alonso JA. Sexual size dimorphism in the Common Crane, a monogamous, plumage-monomorphic bird. Ornis Fenn. 2019;96:194–204.

    Google Scholar 

  60. Francis CM, Wood DS. Effects of age and wear on wing length of wood-warblers. J Field Ornithol. 1989;60(4):495–503.

    Google Scholar 

  61. Merom K, McCleery R, Yom-Tov Y. Age-related changes in wing-length and body mass in the Reed Warbler Acrocephalus scirpaceusand Clamorous Reed Warbler A. stentoreus. Bird Study. 1999;46(2):249–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/00063659909461137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Smith RD. Age determination, wing-feather colour and wing-length change in Snow Buntings Plectrophenax nivalis. Ringing Migr. 1992;13(1):43–51. https://doi.org/10.1080/03078698.1992.9674014.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Jarrett NS, Warren SM. A preliminary guide for age and sex determination of the Houbara Bustard Chlamydotis undulata macqueenii. J-Bombay Nat Hist Soc. 1999;96:28–41.

    Google Scholar 

  64. Adams ES. Approaches to the study of territory size and shape. Annu Rev Ecol Syst. 2001;32(1):277–303. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ecolsys.32.081501.114034.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Searcy WA, Beecher MD. Song as an aggressive signal in songbirds. Anim Behav. 2009;78(6):1281–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2009.08.011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Williams KJ, Wilsey BJ, McNaughton SJ, Banyikwa FF. Temporally variable rainfall does not limit yields of Serengeti grasses. Oikos. 1998;81(3):463. https://doi.org/10.2307/3546768.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. de Miranda JD, Padilla FM, Lázaro R, Pugnaire FI. Do changes in rainfall patterns affect semiarid annual plant communities? Semiarid annual plant communities and climate change. J Veg Sci. 2009;20(2):269–76. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1654-1103.2009.05680.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Oli MK, Jacobson HA, Leopold BD. Pattern of space use by female black bears in the White River National Wildlife Refuge, Arkansas, USA. J Nat Conserv. 2002;10(2):87–93. https://doi.org/10.1078/1617-1381-00010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Smith TR, Pelton, MR. Home ranges and movements of black bears in a bottomland hardwood forest in Arkansas. Bears Biol Manag. 1990;8:213–218.

  70. Koehler GM, Pierce DJ. Black Bear home-range sizes in Washington: Climatic, vegetative, and social influences. J Mammal. 2003;84(1):81–91. https://doi.org/10.1644/1545-1542(2003)084.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Lurz PWW, Garson PJ, Wauters LA. Effects of temporal and spatial variations in food supply on the space and habitat use of red squirrels (Sciurus vulgaris L.). J Zool. 2000;251(2):167–78. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7998.2000.tb00601.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Rodel HG, Stubbe M. Shifts in food availability and associated shifts in space use and diet in stone marten. Lutra. 2006;49(1):67.

    Google Scholar 

  73. Saïd S, Gaillard J-M, Duncan P, Guillon N, Guillon N, Servanty S, et al. Ecological correlates of home-range size in spring–summer for female roe deer (Capreolus capreolus) in a deciduous woodland. J Zool. 2005;267(3):301–8. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0952836905007454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Tufto J, Andersen R, Linnell J. Habitat use and ecological correlates of home range size in a small Cervid: the roe deer. J Anim Ecol. 1996;65(6):715. https://doi.org/10.2307/5670.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Dodd CK. Status, conservation, and management. In: Seigel RA, Collins JT, Novak SS, editors. Snakes: ecology and evolutionary biology. New York: MacMillan; 1987. p. 478–513.

    Google Scholar 

  76. Fraser KC, Davies KT, Davy CM, Ford AT, Flockhart DTT, Martins EG. Tracking the conservation promise of movement ecology. Front Ecol Evol. 2018;6:150. https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2018.00150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Collins D. A study of the Canarian Houbara Bustard (Chlamydotis undulata fuertaventurae) with special reference to its behaviour and ecology. PhD Thesis, University of London, UK, 1984; 119.

  78. Launay F, Loughland R. Breeding system of Houbara Bustard Chlamydotis undulata macqueeni: preliminary observations. Sandgrouse. 1995;35:14–7.

    Google Scholar 

  79. Ponomareva TS. Reproductive behaviour and distribution of Houbara Bustards on their breeding grounds. Zool Zh. 1983;4:592–602.

    Google Scholar 

  80. Hingrat Y, Saint Jalme M, Ysnel F, Le Nuz E, Lacroix F. Habitat use and mating system of the houbara bustard (Chlamydotis undulata undulata) in a semi-desertic area of North Africa: implications for conservation. J Ornithol. 2007;148(1):39–52. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10336-006-0098-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  81. Del Hoyo J, Collar NJ, Christie DA, Elliott A, Fishpool LDC. HBW and BirdLife International Illustrated Checklist of the Birds of the World. 2014.

  82. Gaucher P. Breeding biology of the Houbara Bustard Chlamydotis undulata undulata in Algeria. Alauda. 1995;63:291–8.

    Google Scholar 

  83. Glutz UN, Bauer KM, Bezzel E. Handbuch der Vögel Mitteleuropas. Frankfurt: Akademische Verlagsgesellschaft; 1973. p. 5.

    Google Scholar 

  84. Gubin BM. Methods of counting Asian Houbara Bustard. In: Launay F, Bailey T, editors. Counting Houbara Bustard. Abu Dhabi: National Avian Research Centre, IUCN Species Survival Commission and Birdlife International. 1999. p. 8–12.

  85. Deeming D, Paillat P, Hémon S, Saint JM. Attentiveness and turning patterns during incubation in a houbara bustard (Chlamydotis undulata macqueenii) nest. Avian Poult Biol Rev. 2001;12:182–4.

    Google Scholar 

  86. Labocha MK, Hayes JP. Morphometric estimates of condition in birds. J Ornithol. 2012;153:1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  87. Gosler AG, Greenwood JJD, Baker JK, Davidson NC. The field determination of body size and condition in passerines: a report to the British Ringing Committee. Bird Study. 1998;45(1):92–103. https://doi.org/10.1080/00063659809461082.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  88. Anich NM, Benson TJ, Bednarz JC. Factors influencing home-range size of Swainson’s warblers in eastern Arkansas. Condor. 2010;112(1):149–58. https://doi.org/10.1525/cond.2010.080103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  89. Bautista LM, Alonso JC, Alonso JA. A field test of ideal free distribution in flock-feeding common cranes. J Anim Ecol. 1995;64(6):747. https://doi.org/10.2307/5853.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  90. Bautista LM, Alonso J, Alonso JA. Foraging site displacement in common crane flocks. Anim Behav. 1998;56(5):1237–43. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1998.0882.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Stewart IF. Variation of wing length with age. Bird Study. 1963;10(1):1–9. https://doi.org/10.1080/00063656309476036.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  92. Huete AR. A soil-adjusted vegetation index (SAVI). Remote Sens Environ. 1988;25(3):295–309. https://doi.org/10.1016/0034-4257(88)90106-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  93. Rouse JW, Haas RH, Schell JA, Deering DW. Monitoring vegetation systems in the Great Plains with ERTS. Third ERTS Symposium, Washington DC. 1973;309–317.

  94. Ren H, Zhou G, Zhang F. Using negative soil adjustment factor in soil-adjusted vegetation index (SAVI) for aboveground living biomass estimation in arid grasslands. Remote Sens Environ. 2018;209:439–45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rse.2018.02.068.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  95. Hansteen TL, Andreassen HP, Ims RA. Effects of spatiotemporal scale on autocorrelation and home range estimators. J Wildl Manage. 1997;61(2):280. https://doi.org/10.2307/3802583.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  96. Kernohan BJ, Gitzen RA, Millspaugh JJ. Analysis of animal space use and movements. In: Millspaugh JJ, Marzluff KM, editors. Radio tracking and animal populations. Academic Press. Elsevier; San Diego, California. 2001. p. 125–66.

  97. Seaman DE, Powell RA. An evaluation of the accuracy of kernel density estimators for home range analysis. Ecology. 1996;77(7):2075–85. https://doi.org/10.2307/2265701.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  98. Swihart RK, Slade NA. Testing for independence of observations in animal movements. Ecology. 1985;66(4):1176–84. https://doi.org/10.2307/1939170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  99. De Solla SR, Bonduriansky R, Brooks RJ. Eliminating autocorrelation reduces biological relevance of home range estimates. J Anim Ecol. 1999;68(2):221–34. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2656.1999.00279.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  100. Reynolds TD, Laundre JW. Time intervals for estimating pronghorn and coyote home ranges and daily movements. J Wildl Manage. 1990;54(2):316. https://doi.org/10.2307/3809049.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  101. Blundell GM, Maier JAK, Debevec EM. Linear home ranges: Effects of smoothing, sample size, and autocorrelation on kernel estimates. Ecol Monogr. 2001;71(3):469–89. https://doi.org/10.1890/0012-9615(2001)071[0469:lhreos]2.0.co;2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  102. Fieberg J. Kernel density estimators of home range: smoothing and the autocorrelation red herring. Ecology. 2007;88(4):1059–66. https://doi.org/10.1890/06-0930.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  103. Otis DL, White GC. Autocorrelation of location estimates and the analysis of radiotracking data. J Wildl Manage. 1999;63(3):1039. https://doi.org/10.2307/3802819.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  104. Worton BJ. Kernel methods for estimating the utilization distribution in home-range studies. Ecology. 1989;70(1):164–8. https://doi.org/10.2307/1938423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  105. Mohr CO. Table of equivalent populations of north American small mammals. Am Midl Nat. 1947;37(1):223. https://doi.org/10.2307/2421652.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  106. Smith PE, Southwood TRE. Ecological methods with particular reference to the study of insect populations. Ecology. 1979;60(6):1290. https://doi.org/10.2307/1936977.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  107. Noonan MJ, Tucker MA, Fleming CH, Akre TS, Alberts SC, Ali AH, et al. A comprehensive analysis of autocorrelation and bias in home range estimation. Ecol Monogr. 2019;89(2):e01344. https://doi.org/10.1002/ecm.1344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  108. Dehnad K, Silverman B. Density estimation for statistics and data analysis. Technometrics. 1987;29(4):495. https://doi.org/10.2307/1269475.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  109. Izenman AJ. Recent developments in nonparametric density estimation. J Am Stat Assoc. 1991;86(413):205. https://doi.org/10.2307/2289732.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  110. Millspaugh J, Marzluff JM. Radio tracking and animal populations. San Diego: Academic Press; 2001.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  111. Turlach BA. Bandwidth selection in kernel density estimation: a review. Louvain-la-Neuve: Universite Catholique de Louvain; 1993.

    Google Scholar 

  112. R Development Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2012) at http://www.r-project.org.

  113. Signer J, Balkenhol N. Reproducible home ranges (rhr): a new, user-friendly R package for analyses of wildlife telemetry data: reproducible Home-Range Analysis With R. Wildl Soc Bull. 2015;39(2):358–63. https://doi.org/10.1002/wsb.539.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  114. Kie JG, Matthiopoulos J, Fieberg J, Powell RA, Cagnacci F, Mitchell MS, et al. The home range concept: are traditional estimators still relevant with modern telemetry technology? Phil Trans R Soc B. 2010;365:2221–31.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  115. Kie JG. A rule-based ad hoc method for selecting a bandwidth in kernel home-range analyses. Anim Biotelemetry. 2013;1(1):13. https://doi.org/10.1186/2050-3385-1-13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  116. Schuler KL, Schroeder GM, Jenks JA, Kie JG. Ad hoc smoothing parameter performance in kernel estimates of GPS-derived home ranges. Wildlife Biol. 2014;20(5):259–66. https://doi.org/10.2981/wlb.12117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  117. Zuur AF, Ieno EN, Walker NJ, Saveliev AA, Smith GM. Mixed effects models and extensions in ecology with R. New York: Springer; 2009. p. 574.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  118. Burnham KP, Anderson DR. Model selection and multimodel inference: a practical information-theoretic approach. Nueva York: Springer; 2010.

    Google Scholar 

  119. Burnham KP, Anderson DR, Huyvaert KP. AIC model selection and multimodel inference in behavioral ecology: some background, observations, and comparisons. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2011;65(1):23–35. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-010-1029-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  120. Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting linear mixed effects models using lme4. J Stat Softw. 2015;67:1–48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  121. Nathan R, Spiegel O, Fortmann-Roe S, Harel R, Wikelski M, Getz WM. Using tri-axial acceleration data to identify behavioral modes of free-ranging animals: general concepts and tools illustrated for griffon vultures. J Exp Biol. 2012;215(Pt 6):986–96. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.058602.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  122. Resheff YS, Rotics S, Harel R, Spiegel O, Nathan R. AcceleRater: a web application for supervised learning of behavioral modes from acceleration measurements. Mov Ecol. 2014;2(1):27. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40462-014-0027-0.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  123. Jiménez-Valverde A. Prevalence affects the evaluation of discrimination capacity in presence-absence species distribution models. Biodivers Conserv. 2021;30(5):1331–40. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10531-021-02144-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  124. Barton K. MuMIn: Multi-Model Inference. R Package Version 1.15.6. 2016.

  125. Hardouin LA, Nevoux M, Robert A, Gimenez O, Lacroix F, Hingrat Y. Determinants and costs of natal dispersal in a lekking species. Oikos. 2012;121(6):804–12. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0706.2012.20313.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  126. Margalida A, Pérez-García JM, Afonso I, Moreno-Opo R. Spatial and temporal movements in Pyrenean bearded vultures (Gypaetus barbatus): Integrating movement ecology into conservation practice. Sci Rep. 2016;6:35746. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep35746.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  127. Penteriani V, Ferrer M, Delgado MM. Floater strategies and dynamics in birds, and their importance in conservation biology: towards an understanding of nonbreeders in avian populations: conservation biology needs floaters. Anim Conserv. 2011;14(3):233–41. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-1795.2010.00433.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  128. Hellmich J. The display run of the Houbara Bustard Chlamydotis undulata fuertaventurae. Ornithologische Beobachter. 2003;100(2):127–42.

    Google Scholar 

  129. Alonso JC, Abril-Colón I, Palacín C, Ucero A, Álvarez-Martínez JM. Maximizing sexual signal transmission: use of multiple display sites by male houbara bustards. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2022;76(9):1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  130. Lesobre L, Lacroix F, Le Nuz E, Hingrat Y, Chalah T, Jaime MS. Absence of male reproductive skew, along with high frequency of polyandry and conspecific brood parasitism in the lekking Houbara bustard Chlamydotis undulata undulata. J Avian Biol. 2010;41(2):117–27. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-048x.2009.04794.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  131. Chargé R, Teplitsky C, Hingrat Y, Saint Jalme M, Lacroix F, Sorci G. Quantitative genetics of sexual display, ejaculate quality and size in a lekking species. J Anim Ecol. 2013;82(2):399–407. https://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2656.12023.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  132. Jacquet JM. Seasonal changes in food intake and body mass in captive houbara bustards (Chlamydotis undulata) and effect of ambient temperature. J Arid Environ. 1998;38(3):359–70. https://doi.org/10.1006/jare.1997.0349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  133. Saint Jalme M, Williams J, Mickaelian I, Paillat P. Seasonal variation of LH, sex steroids, body mass, molt, display, and laying in two subspecies of Houbara bustard, Chlamydotis undulata macqueenii and Chlamydotis undulata undulata, housed in outdoor cages under natural conditions. Gen Comp Endocrinol. 1996;102(1):102–12. https://doi.org/10.1006/gcen.1996.0051.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  134. Cadet A. Les signaux liés aux parades sexuelles chez l’Outarde houbara sont-ils des indicateurs honnêtes de l’état de santé des oiseaux? MSc thesis. Université Paris-Sud, Paris. 2007.

  135. Chargé R, Saint Jalme M, Lacroix F, Cadet A, Sorci G. Male health status, signalled by courtship display, reveals ejaculate quality and hatching success in a lekking species. J Anim Ecol. 2010;79(4):843–50. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2656.2010.01696.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  136. Díaz-Valera T. Cuantificación de estimadores de éxito reproductivo en la avutarda hubara canaria (Chlamydotis undulata fuertaventurae). Trabajo fin de Máster. Máster en Biología Avanzada. Sevilla. 2019.

  137. Illera JC, Díaz M. Reproduction in an endemic bird of a semiarid island: a food-mediated process. J Avian Biol. 2006;37(5):447–56. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2006.0908-8857.03676.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  138. Immelmann K. Ecological aspects of periodic reproduction. In: Farner DS, King JR, editors. Avian biology, vol. 1. New York: Academic Press; 1973. p. 341–89.

    Google Scholar 

  139. Morrison SA, Bolger DT. Variation in a sparrow’s reproductive success with rainfall: food and predator-mediated processes. Oecologia. 2002;133(3):315–24. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00442-002-1040-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  140. Yosef R, Zduniak P. Variation in clutch size, egg size variability and reproductive output in the Desert Finch (Rhodospiza obsoleta). J Arid Environ. 2008;72(9):1631–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaridenv.2008.03.019.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  141. Dawson A. Control of the annual cycle in birds: endocrine constraints and plasticity in response to ecological variability. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2008;363(1497):1621–33. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2007.0004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  142. Combreau O, Gelinaud G, Smith TR. Home range and movements of houbara bustards introduced in the Najd Pediplain in Saudi Arabia. J Arid Environ. 2000;44(2):229–40. https://doi.org/10.1006/jare.1999.0578.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  143. Mendelssohn, H. Development of houbara (Chlamydotis undulata) populations in Israel and captive breeding. In: Proceedings of the symposium on the Houbara Bustard Chlamydotis undulata, Athens, Greece. 1980; 131–139.

  144. Étchécopar RD, Hüe F. Les oiseaux de Chine, de Mongolie et de Coree, Papeete, Tahiti: Editions du Pacifique. 1978.

  145. Collins DR. The diet of the houbara bustard Chlamydotis undulata fuertaventurae in the Canary Islands. Bol Mus Munic Funchal. 1993;2:57–67.

    Google Scholar 

  146. Medina FM. Foraging use of cultivated fields by the Houbara Bustard Chlamydotis undulata fuertaventurae. Rothschild and Hartert, 1894 on Fuerteventura (Canary Islands). Bird Conserv Int. 1999;9(4):373–86. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0959270900003531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  147. Ucero A, Alonso JC, Palacin C, Abril-Colón I, Álvarez-Martínez JM. Display site selection in a ground dwelling bird: the importance of viewshed. Behavioral Ecology, under review. 2022.

  148. Alonso JC, Álvarez-Martínez JM, Palacín C. Leks in ground-displaying birds: hotspots or safe places? Behav Ecol. 2012;23(3):491–501. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arr215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  149. Magaña M, Alonso JC, Martín CA, Bautista LM, Martín B. Nest-site selection by Great Bustards Otis tarda suggests a trade-off between concealment and visibility: nesting habitat selection by Great Bustards. Ibis. 2010;152(1):77–89. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1474-919x.2009.00976.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  150. Alonso JC, Palacín C, Alonso JA, Martín CA. Post-breeding migration in male great bustards: low tolerance of the heaviest Palaearctic bird to summer heat. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2009;63(12):1705–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  151. Islam MZ, Singh A, Basheer MP, Judas J, Boug A. Differences in space use and habitat selection between captive-bred and wild-born houbara bustards in Saudi Arabia: results from a long-term reintroduction program. J Zool. 2013;289(4):251–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  152. Moreira F, Morgado R, Arthur S. Great bustard Otis tarda habitat selection in relation to agricultural use in southern Portugal. Wildl Biol. 2004;10(4):251–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  153. Morgado R, Moreira F. Seasonal population dynamics, nest site selection, sex-ratio and clutch size of the Great Bustard Otis tarda in two adjacent lekking areas. Ardeola. 2000;47(2):237–46.

    Google Scholar 

  154. Palacin C, Alonso JC, Martin CA, Alonso JA. The importance of traditional farmland areas for steppe birds: a case study of migrant female Great Bustards Otis tarda in Spain. Ibis. 2012;154(1):85–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  155. Delgado MP, Traba J, de la Morena ELG, Morales MB. Habitat selection and density-dependent relationships in spatial occupancy by male Little Bustards Tetrax tetrax. Ardea. 2010;98(2):185–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  156. Martínez C, de Juana E. El Sison. La Garcilla. 1995; 92: 16-19.

  157. Aghainajafi-Zadeh S, Hemami MR, Karami M, Dolman PM. Wintering habitat use by houbara bustard (Chlamydotis macqueenii) in steppes of Harat, central Iran. J Arid Environ. 2010;74(8):912–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaridenv.2010.01.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  158. Faaborg J, Baltz ME, Gram KK. Non-breeding season considerations for the conservation of migratory. Manag Midwest Landsc Conserv Neotrop Migr Birds. 1996;187:189.

    Google Scholar 

  159. Tucker GM, Heath MF. Birds in Europe: their conservation status. BirdLife International, Cambridge, UK (BirdLife Conservation Series no. 3). 1994.

  160. Börger L, Franconi N, Ferretti F, Meschi F, De Michele G, Gantz A, et al. An integrated approach to identify spatiotemporal and individual-level determinants of animal home range size. Am Nat. 2006;168(4):471–85. https://doi.org/10.1086/507883.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  161. Krebs JR. Territory and breeding density in the great tit, Parus major L. Ecology. 1971;52(1):2–22. https://doi.org/10.2307/1934734.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  162. Sanderson GC. The study of mammal movements: a review. J Wildl Manage. 1966;30(1):215. https://doi.org/10.2307/3797914.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  163. Wilson EO. Sociobiology: the new synthesis. Inglaterra: Harvard University Press; 1975.

    Google Scholar 

  164. Höglund J, Alatalo RV. Leks. Princeton: Princeton University Press; 1995.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  165. Ligon JD. The evolution of avian breeding systems. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1999.

    Google Scholar 

  166. Morales MB, Jiguet F, Arroyo B. Exploded leks: what bustards can teach us. Ardeola. 2001;48(1):85–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  167. Relyea RA, Lawrence RK, Demarais S. Home range of desert mule deer: testing the body-size and habitat-productivity hypotheses. J Wildl Manage. 2000;64(1):146. https://doi.org/10.2307/3802984.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to A. Rodríguez, T. Díaz-Valera and F. Corujo for their collaboration during the fieldwork, E. Arrondo for helping with the AcceleRater software, and A. Perona for his help and tips about the “rhr package”, M. A. Cabrera for managing capture permits and for his support during the project, F. Crespo, M. Armas, A. Ibarguren and M. Gil for their assistance during the preparation of the project, the guards of the Cabildo of Lanzarote, Timanfaya National Park and La Graciosa, including TRAGSA personnel, the veterinarian C. M. Suárez, and all authorities of the Government of the Canary Islands for their collaboration. We also thank J. M. Poveda and F. Suárez of GRAFCAN for providing images and ortophotos of the islands. Two anonymous reviewers and the associate editor provided useful comments that helped to improve our manuscript. This study was funded by Red Eléctrica de España (REE) through a contract with Agencia Estatal Consejo Superior de Investigaciones Científicas (CSIC).

Funding

This study was funded by Red Eléctrica de España (REE) through a contract with Agencia Estatal Consejo Superior de Investigaciones Científicas (CSIC). AU was supported by a predoctoral contract from the Ministry of Science, Innovation and Universities.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

IAC: Data curation (lead); Field work (equal). Formal analysis (lead); Investigation (equal); Methodology (equal); Writing-original draft (lead). JCA: Conceptualization (lead); Data curation (supporting); Field work (lead); Investigation (equal); Project administration (lead); Funding acquisition (lead); Supervision (lead); Formal analysis (supporting); Methodology (lead); Writing-original draft (equal). CP: Conceptualization (equal); Data curation (supporting); Field work (equal); Investigation (supporting); Methodology (equal). AU: Data curation (supporting); Field work (equal); Investigation (supporting); Resources (supporting). JMAM: Data curation (supporting); Formal analysis (supporting). All authors contributed critically to the drafts and gave final approval for publication.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Inmaculada Abril-Colón.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent for participation

Capture, handling and marking houbara bustards was authorized and conducted under permissions issued by regional authorities (Viceconsejería de Medio Ambiente, Gobierno de Canarias, license 2015/10584). The weight of the logger plus harness material was below the commonly accepted limit of 5% of the weight of the birds. We did not observe stress signs in the birds as a result of marking. The methods used comply with the Spanish guidelines for ethical use in animal research.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1

. Supplementary tables and figures of Factors modulating home range and resource use: a case study with Canarian houbara bustards

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Abril-Colón, I., Alonso, J.C., Palacín, C. et al. Factors modulating home range and resource use: a case study with Canarian houbara bustards. Mov Ecol 10, 49 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40462-022-00346-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40462-022-00346-1

Keywords