Skip to main content

Differential fall migratory routes of adult and juvenile Ipswich Sparrows (Passerculus sandwichensis princeps)

Abstract

Background

Island breeding birds present an ideal system for studying migratory movements in passerines because their populations are clearly demarcated, and individuals must depart on migration from a single location. The Ipswich Sparrow (Paserculus sandwichensis princeps) is a subspecies of the Savannah Sparrow that breeds exclusively on Sable Island, Nova Scotia, Canada and winters along the Atlantic coast of North America. We used a network of 34 automated VHF telemetry receivers to track radio-tagged adult and juvenile Ipswich Sparrows from their breeding island southward through the first half of their fall migratory journey.

Results

We compared adult to juvenile timing and routes. We show that juveniles leave the island approximately 24 days prior to adults and remain temporally separated from them during migration through Nova Scotia. Juveniles have different overwater orientations that result in migratory routes with shorter ocean crossings and a longer overall distance travelled compared to adults. Juveniles also have more frequent and longer stopovers, and displayed some reverse migration.

Conclusion

We demonstrate that migratory routes differ between adults and juveniles, suggesting that routes change as individuals age, possibly through learning or social interactions. These differential routes also suggest that sparrows experience risk in different ways with juveniles selecting shorter overwater flights with less navigational risk at the cost of increased time spent in migration.

Background

In passerine birds, it has been suggested that 85 % of annual mortality can occur during the migration season [1, 2] which suggests that there should be strong selection for adaptations to reduce mortality during that period. Selection could act on the timing of migratory flights, the choice of stopover sites, and the choice of migratory routes. Under optimal migration theory, these decisions are based on trade-offs between the need to accumulate energy at high-quality stopover sites, to avoid predation, and to select for good weather conditions [3, 4]. What constitutes an optimal migratory route appears to vary between species. For example, some passerines appear to minimize time costs [5], while others minimize energetic costs [6].

Adult and juvenile songbirds have different experience and abilities [7, 8]. It can therefore be expected that their perception of the relative risks and benefits of options during migration may also differ. Compared to adults, juveniles may be less able to avoid predators [9] and orientate in appropriate directions [10], have more variability in their orientation [11], and take longer and more frequent stopovers [12, 13]. In addition, both adults and juveniles are known to undertake flights in inappropriate migratory directions in both spring and fall [1416]. Such ‘reverse migrations’ and ‘landscape-scale stopover movements’ may be due to orientation errors, overshooting targets, or for gathering information about the environment [17, 18].

There is considerable support for the hypothesis that during migration, juvenile migratory songbirds orient using magnetic and celestial cues [1921]. Translocation experiments suggest that it is primarily adults who can re-orient en route, possibly due to a learned navigational map based on magnetic cues [10, 22]. Thus, as they age and gain experience, individuals can correct ‘mistakes’ in their initial migrations by altering routes that optimize trade-offs—for example, avoiding barriers, more rapidly locating suitable stopover sites, and taking better advantage of weather to facilitate movement.

Behavioural differences between adults and juveniles may also be magnified when ecological barriers such as oceans are encountered [23, 24]. Long overwater flights present a challenge to migratory songbirds since they must acquire the necessary energy reserves to complete a non-stop flight as well as orient correctly [15, 23, 25]. Thus, small differences in adult and juvenile ability and experience could result in large observable differences in behaviour when open water is encountered en route. Altered behaviour at open water could include longer stopovers, differential route choice [26], or movements to locations with more food and fewer predators [14, 25, 27]. Because they have experience, it thus follows that adults may display less variability in orientation, fewer reverse migration or landscape-scale stopover movements en route, and less time at stopovers at ecological barriers.

We studied an endemic island passerine that undergoes a yearly migration typical of most short-distance temperate migrants. The Ipswich Sparrow (Passerculus sandwichensis princeps) is a subspecies of Savannah Sparrow that breeds exclusively on Sable Island, Nova Scotia, Canada and winters in coastal dunes along the eastern seaboard of North America [28, 29]. Ipswich Sparrows migrating south in the fall must cross or avoid two large water bodies (Fig. 1). Their options are to fly northwest to the Nova Scotia mainland and travel directly west to either cross or circumvent the Bay of Fundy (route a), travel south along the southwest coast of Nova Scotia to cross the Gulf of Maine (route b), or migrate directly overwater from Sable Island to Cape Cod, MA (route c). Observations during fall migration (www.ebird.ca) indicate that at least some individuals are detected along the Bay of Fundy suggesting partially overland routes through Nova Scotia. Which individuals select these routes is unknown, but we propose that age may help explain the observed differential routes because of the aforementioned differences in relative risks and benefits of overwater flights.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Map of telemetry array and potential migratory routes of Ipswich Sparrows. Potential migratory routes of Ipswich Sparrows including route a; land-based (green line), route b; coastal across the Gulf of Maine (blue line), and route c; direct to Cape Cod (orange line). Red circles represent receiver sites. Bon Portage Island = BPI

Using automated digital telemetry [25] we compared the timing of initiation of migration, the routes chosen, and the behavioural decisions en route (stopover behaviours, decisions at two major water crossings, extent of ‘reverse’ migration) of adult and juvenile Ipswich Sparrows. The differences allow us to infer to what extent passerines learn and adjust their routes as they age.

Methods

Study sites and methods

Sable Island (44°N, 60° W) is situated off the coast of Nova Scotia, approximately 160 km from the nearest mainland, in the Northern Atlantic Ocean. It is a roughly 42 km long crescent shaped sandbar that is 1.5 km across at its widest point. Just under half the surface is covered in vegetation comprising shrubs, forbs, and grasses [28].

We radio tagged 64 Ipswich Sparrows (16 adult male, 15 adult female, 33 fledglings) between 12–18 August 2013 on Sable Island. To improve the odds that tracked juveniles would survive until the onset of migration [30] only early brood juveniles (those likely from the first or second broods) were tagged.

Ipswich Sparrows winter along Atlantic coast of North America with the highest densities being found between New Jersey and Virginia. They migrate north to arrive on Sable Island in late April and begin fall migration in late September [28]. They can have up to 4 broods, so by mid-August, early broods may be 2–3 months old, capable of flight and fully independent.

Individuals were trapped with mist nets and affixed with NTQB-3-2 Avian Nano Tag coded transmitters (0.67 g; 124 day expected tag life; Lotek Wireless Inc., Newmarket, ON) using a leg-loop harness [31]. Sparrows have an average mass of 22.5 g so tag weight did not exceed 3 % of their total body weight [32]. We saw no evidence of altered behaviour or mortality several days after tagging.

Four automated telemetry towers (14 antennas) with SensorGnome receivers (www.sensorgnome.org) were operated on Sable Island during the period of the study. An additional 30 receiver stations were situated on the Atlantic coast between Northern Nova Scotia and Cape Cod, Massachusetts (Fig. 1). Although not all of these towers were operational during the entire study (see Additional file 1 for details on towers) a sufficient number of them were present and operational to detect individuals moving through the northern and southern parts of the Gulf of Maine. Receivers in Nova Scotia towers were situated between 20–50 km apart along the coast in elevated areas with unobstructed views of the ocean.

Analysis

Data initially recorded by SensorGnomes contain a proportion of false positives in order to reduce the number of missed detections. Data were post-processed following methods described by [16, 33]. We initially included only sets of 3 consecutive detections of a tag that were separated by the specific burst interval for each tag. We then specifically examined some sets of 2 subsequent detections, and included two of these sets where the location of the detection was consistent with the movement of the bird (as determined by the sets of 3 subsequent detections).

Although both adult males and females were tagged we could not make comparisons between the sexes because only 11 adults were detected on the mainland. Furthermore, we did not assess mass or fat content differences between individuals because birds were tagged at least one month prior to departure, and so measurements would not reflect the true measures at the time of departure.

To determine the timing of initiation of migration, we used the date of first detection on mainland Nova Scotia, which assumes that the initiation of migration is in fact the departure of an individual from Sable Island. Most individuals (27/39) were detected on the mainland within 12 h of their final detections on Sable Island, and 37/39 were detected within 100 h of departure. We similarly calculated the date of final detection in Nova Scotia.

We calculated bearings for two types of overwater flights, those from Sable Island to mainland Nova Scotia and those from the southernmost point in mainland Nova Scotia (Bon Portage Island; BPI; Fig. 1) across the Gulf of Maine. We calculated the rhumb-line bearing (package geosphere [34]) between the locations of the respective towers at the ends of the presumed flight path. Bearings were not calculated for birds with final detections at stations to the northeast of BPI because they may have taken overland routes which would not provide true overwater bearings. Bearings were only calculated for individuals that were first detected <100 h after their last detection prior to crossing the water body. Most individuals were detected within 12 h of departing Sable Island; for those that did not, we assume that they reached the respective mainland and initiated a stopover. Two individuals arrived 9 and 18 days after their last detection (longer than the average stopover length) and so were removed because there is considerable uncertainty about what they did in the intervening time. To classify a flight as direct or not, we calculated ground speed (the rhumb-line distance between two consecutive detections at two different sites divided by the time between detections).

When an individual was detected at a receiver to the northeast of a previous detection, we considered it to have made a ‘reverse migration’. When an individual was detected at two adjacent receivers > 10 h apart, or where detections at the same receiver spanned > 8 h with no detections at other receivers, we considered an individual to have ‘stopped over’. Detection range varies considerably depending primarily on an individual’s behaviour; those on the ground are only detected within a range of a few km, whereas those in flight can be routinely detected up to 15 km distant. Distances between adjacent receivers in Nova Scotia were between 9 and 48 km so at the majority of sites an individual would not be detected while stationary at a stopover, but would be detected upon resuming migration. We excluded detections at Cape Cod from consideration for stopover, where most detections spanned >16 days (compared to a maximum of 10 days at all other sites; Table 1). This clear difference in stopover behaviour, and that the location is within the wintering area of Ipswich Sparrows, suggests that these individuals had completed their migration. Thus, they were excluded to ensure stopover analyses did not include overwintering birds that had ceased migrating.

Table 1 Mean number and duration of stopovers, and flight distance between stopovers of adult and juvenile Ispwich Sparrows detected on the mainland

We fit generalized linear models (glms) to test the hypothesis that the timing of initiation of migration (glm with normal errors) and the number of stopovers (glm with Poisson errors) depended on age. For stopover duration, there were multiple responses for an individual (5/11 adults and 22/28 juveniles had >1 stopover) so we fit generalized linear mixed models (normal errors) with individual as a random effect (package lme4 [35]) to test the hypothesis that stopover duration depended on age. In the results we present the coefficient for the age effect, using ‘adult’ as the reference state.

For analysis of the bearings of individuals crossing water, we used circular analysis of variance (package circular [36]) to test whether the means of the bearings depended on age and Watson’s two-sample test of homogeneity (package CircStats [37]) to test whether the variance of bearings depended on age. For two-by-two contingency tables we used Fisher’s Exact Test for count data.

Results

Ipswich Sparrows remained on Sable Island for 31–86 days after tagging. On the mainland we detected 28/33 (85 %) of juveniles but only 11/31 (30 %) of adults. All individuals detected on the mainland were initially detected in Nova Scotia (Table 2); 29 of these (4 adults, 25 juveniles) were subsequently detected in coastal areas of the Gulf of Maine.

Table 2 Total number of tagged Ipswich Sparrows detected by location on the mainland

Timing of the first migratory flight

Juveniles (n = 28) were first detected on the mainland between 17 September and 23 October (\( \overline{x} = 3 \) October). Adults (n = 11) were first detected on the mainland between 18 October and 11 November (\( \overline{x} = 27 \) October), ~24 days after juveniles (Gaussian glm; date of initiation of migration; β (juveniles in relation to adults) = −24.0 ± 3.15, p < 0.001). Adults and juveniles remained temporally separate during migration through Nova Scotia (Gaussian glm; date of last detection in mainland Nova Scotia; β (juveniles in relation to adults) = −20.8 ± 2.79, p < 0.001).

Selection of route

There was evidence of different routes between adults (Fig. 2a) and juveniles (Fig. 2b) based on overwater orientations, and points of detection along the coast of Nova Scotia and along the Gulf of Maine region. All 39 tracked migratory journeys began with an initial overwater flight between Sable Island and mainland Nova Scotia. Most of these (28/39; 72 %) were detected within the first 12 h suggesting that the receiver array was effective in detecting direct overwater flights between Sable Island and initial mainland landfall. The angle of orientation of these initial flights differed slightly between adults and juveniles (288° vs. 299°; circular analysis of variance; p = 0.03) but there was no evidence for differences in variability (rho = 0.018 vs. 0.026; Watson’s two-sample test for homogeneity = 0.05, p > 0.1).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Map of adult and juvenile Ipswich Sparrow migratory routes. Observed routes of (a) adult (n = 11) and (b) juvenile (n = 28) Ipswich Sparrows. Direct flights by two juveniles across mainland Nova Scotia indicated by blue lines. Receiver stations denoted by red circles. Bon Portage Island = BPI

After reaching mainland Nova Scotia, most individuals were detected travelling southwest along the southern coastline. The adults (n = 4, Fig. 2a) that were detected in coastal Gulf of Maine were last detected in Nova Scotia in the extreme southwest site (BPI) suggesting that they flew across the Gulf of Maine. About half (12/25) of the juveniles detected in the Gulf of Maine were last detected in Nova Scotia at BPI, the rest (13/25) were last detected at 5 different sites in coastal Nova Scotia ~50 to 230 km to the northeast of BPI. These individuals likely flew via a partially overland route that circumvented all or part of the Gulf of Maine. Two of these individuals were detected on the northern Gulf of Maine < 8 h after their last detections in Nova Scotia (ground speeds of 10 and 13 m/s). These speeds suggest that the flights were direct, and thus primarily overland across mainland Nova Scotia with a short over-water crossing at the Bay of Fundy (Fig. 2b). The other 10 juveniles detected on the Gulf of Maine were observed 1–21 days after their last detections in Nova Scotia; the lack of detections at other sites along the southwest coast of Nova Scotia suggests that they too made flights crossing mainland Nova Scotia and the Bay of Fundy to reach the upper Gulf of Maine. Of the remaining 10 individuals that were never detected on the Gulf of Maine, BPI was the most frequent last site of detection (3/7 adults, 3/3 juveniles) suggesting possible direct over-water flights terminating south of Cape Cod, the point furthest south in our telemetry array, or unsuccessful flights across open water.

A significantly higher proportion of juveniles than adults were detected on the Gulf of Maine (89 % vs. 36 %; p = 0.01, Fisher’s exact test; p = 0.002). Although we have no evidence that the proportion of adults and juveniles detected at the northerly Gulf of Maine sites (adults n = 0, juveniles n = 12) differed from that at the more southerly ones (adults n = 4, juveniles n = 13; Fisher’s exact test; p = 0.12) our power to detect a difference was low. We point out however that none of the four adults detected in the Gulf of Maine were detected at the more northerly receiver stations. Furthermore, adults departing from BPI had a significantly more southerly orientation across the Gulf of Maine than did juveniles (252° vs. 282°; circular analysis of variance; p = 0.01). There was no evidence that the variability in bearings differed by age (rho = 0.026 vs. 0.051; Watson’s two sample test for homogeneity = 0. 09, p > 0.1).

Migratory behaviour and stopover

Adults had fewer (1.6 ± 0.36 vs. 3.8 ± 0.36; poisson glm; β = 0.49 ± 0.24, p = 0.037) and shorter (2.8 ± 1.03 vs. 5.8 ± 0.71 days; Gaussian glm; β = 2.8 ± 1.11, p < 0.03) stopovers than juveniles (Table 1). There were more stopovers at BPI than elsewhere in Nova Scotia (19 vs. 5.3 ± 1.03, poisson glm; β = 1.66 ± 0.33, p < 0.001) but their lengths were similar (Gaussian glm; β = 0.03 ± 0.28 days, p = 0.92).

All 3 direction reversals were by juveniles. Two occurred towards the end of a southward migratory flight just prior to a stopover event, and the third occurred at the initiation of a southward migratory flight after a 17 day stopover. The distance between the two consecutive detections comprising the direction reversal ranged from 18–34 km.

Discussion

During fall migration juvenile Ipswich Sparrows departed 24 days prior to adults, had more and longer stopovers, and avoided overwater flights. Adult Ipswich sparrows undertook longer overwater flights than juveniles (both from Sable Island to mainland Nova Scotia and from southwest Nova Scotia across the Gulf of Maine) and some juveniles, but no adults, moved across mainland Nova Scotia to reach the northern Gulf of Maine—crossing or circumnavigating the Bay of Fundy. Collectively, these results suggest that compared to adults, juvenile Ipswich Sparrows spend more time in a migratory state and have fewer and shorter over-water crossings in their fall migrations. One plausible hypothesis that accounts for these differences is that the initial migratory route of Ipswich Sparrows is innate and that as individuals age, these routes change [22].

Age-specific differences in migratory routes have rarely been shown in birds [26, 38, 39] and even more rarely for passerines [40]. Blackpoll warblers from southwest Nova Scotia show dramatically different post-fledging movements that appear to result in differing migratory routes [40]. Juvenile honey buzzards follow a more direct route than adults [26] whereas juvenile sharp-tailed sandpipers take a longer, less direct route [38]. In all cases juveniles are likely following an ancestral route and alternative (learned) routes may have inherent differences in weather conditions, predation, or food availability that make them more attractive [4143]. Since juvenile Ipswich Sparrows depart nearly a month earlier than adults, it is also possible that different routes arise because of inherent differences in the timing of departure from Sable Island related to different physiological needs. For example, adults may leave later than juveniles because they must moult or because they require additional energy after breeding.

Route choice may also be attributable to different perceptions of risk. Routes with longer overwater flights likely have increased risks associated with orientation, weather, and fuel loads, but lowered risks associated with shorter routes (less time in unfamiliar habitats and less exposure to poor weather, predators and parasites [44, 45]). Although we cannot directly assess the actual risks of the routes selected by individuals, one hypothesis that would support the observation that juveniles take routes that reduce or eliminate water crossings is that there has been selection to reduce navigational risk in the first migratory journey and(or) to reduce time spent migrating in the second and subsequent journeys [46]. Observations of foraging behaviour and predator presence at stopover, and finer-scale data on actual routes selected will ultimately be required to tease apart the relative importance of these factors in determining route selection.

Both adults and juveniles were more likely to stopover when they encountered the Gulf of Maine which may suggest a behavioural response to the presence of the Gulf of Maine. If so, the fact the lengths of stopovers at the Gulf of Maine did not vary from the length at other locations, suggests that the amount of fat required for that crossing is not greater than that required for other migratory flights. Thus, if there is a perceived risk to the Gulf of Maine, it is likely related to navigational errors, and not to fuel load. Individuals are known to reduce the risks of crossing large water bodies by remaining at, or returning to, the mainland and subsequently initiating water crossings early in the night, with better body condition, or under better weather conditions [23, 25].

Juveniles had significantly more, and longer stopover events than adults indicating that stopover behaviour may also relate to energy requirements, abilities, and experience that differ with age. Individuals arriving at stopovers in lower energetic condition may stay longer in order to accumulate adequate energy reserves [23]. Furthermore, only juveniles were observed moving in directions along the coast opposite to that expected for the season. Two of these reverse flights occurred immediately before a stopover event, suggesting that these individuals were searching for a stopover site with better foraging opportunities, less competition or fewer predators [47]. The third reversal occurred after a 17 days stopover which is more consistent with landscape-scale stopover movements that have been observed in other species [16, 48]. The lack of such movements by adults suggests an awareness of suitable stopover locations and/or comparatively better body condition than juveniles [49, 50].

We detected the majority of juveniles but many fewer adults leaving Sable Island. The possible reasons for the discrepancy include differential tag loss, post-breeding mortality, or direct adult migratory movements beyond the southern edge of the telemetry array. Demographic studies of passerines generally show much higher rates of yearly mortality for juveniles than adults [51], but it is difficult to determine mortality during post-fledging and migration in small passerines. It is possible that adult Ipswich sparrows suffer increased mortality immediately post-breeding [52]. Unlike many passerines at these latitudes, Ipswich sparrows may have as many as 4 broods in a season [28]. Such high reproductive output may directly cause increased mortality or indirectly exacerbate ‘tag effects’ also resulting in increased mortality. Alternatively, weight losses associated with breeding and moulting could increase the likelihood that adults lose their tags relative to juveniles.

We suspect that adults present in coastal Nova Scotia but not detected in the Gulf of Maine flew directly to make landfall at points farther south than the southern extent of the telemetry array, and so were never again detected. It follows therefore that some adults may have flown directly from Sable Island to more southern wintering grounds than our array could detect. Continued study with increased receiver coverage farther south will aid in determining the fate of this large portion of missing adults.

Conclusion

Due to difficulties in tracking small songbirds, differences in routes between age classes have not been previously shown. Our results suggest that juvenile Ipswich Sparrows may initially select routes based on innate knowledge and may alter routes in following years based on experience or social learning [22]. While differences in routes and stopover behaviour may be attributable to differences in experience, condition, and temporal difference, both adults and juveniles are more likely to stopover at water barriers than at other sites, suggesting over-water crossings are perceived as risky to both age classes.

Availability of supporting data

The data set supporting the results of this article is available in the Motus repository, [http://www.motus-wts.org].

Abbreviations

BPI:

Bon Portage Island

References

  1. Sillett TS, Holmes RT. Variation in survivorship of a migratory songbird throughout its annual cycle. J Anim Ecol. 2002; doi:10.1046/j.1365-2656.2002.00599.x.

  2. Blackburn E, Cresswell W. High within-winter and annual survival rates in a declining Afro-Palaearctic migratory bird suggest that wintering conditions do not limit populations. Ibis. 2015; doi:110.1111/ibi.12319.

  3. Alerstam T, Lindström Å. Optimal bird migration: the relative importance of time, energy, and safety. In: Gwinner E, editor. Bird Migration: the physiology and ecophysiology. Berlin: Springer; 1990. p. 331–51.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  4. Alerstam T, Hedenström A, Åkesson A. Long-distance migration: evoloution and determinants. Oikos. 2003;103:247–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Hedenström A. Adaptations to migration in birds: behavioural strategies, morphology and scaling effects. Phil Trans R Soc B. 2008;363:287–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Schmaljohann H, Korner-Nievergelt F, Naef-Daenzer B, Nagel R, Maggini I, Bulte M. Stopover optimization in a long-distance migrant: the role of fuel load and nocturnal take-off time in Alaskan northern wheatears (Oenanthe oenanthe). Front Zool. 2013; doi:10.1186/1742-9994-10-26.

  7. Jones J, Francis CM, Drew M, Fuller S, Ng MWS. Age-related differences in body mass and rates of mass gain of passerines during autumn migratory stopover. Condor. 2002;104:49-58.

  8. Heise CD, Moore FR. Age-related differences in foraging efficiency, molt, and fat deposition of Gray Catbirds prior to autumn migration. Condor. 2003;105:496–504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Dierschke V. High profit at high risk for juvenile Dunlins Calidris alpina stopping over at Helgoland (German Bight). Ardea. 1998;86:59–69.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Perdeck AC. Two types of orientation in migrating Starlings, Sturnus vulgaris L. and Chaffinches, Fringilla coelebs L., as revealed by displacement experiments. Ardea. 1958;46:1–37.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Moore FR. Age-dependent variability in the migratory orientation of the Savannah Sparrow (Passerculus sandwichensis). Auk. 1984;101:875–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Yong W, Finch DM, Moore RF, Kelly JF. Stopover ecology and habitat use of migratory Wilson’s Warblers. Auk. 1998;115:829–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Rguibi-Idrissi H, Julliard R, Bairlein F. Variation in the stopover duration of Reed Warblers Acrocephalus scipaceus in Morocco: effects of season, age and site. Ibis. 2003;145:650–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Alerstam T. Reoriented bird migration in coastal areas: dispersal to suitable resting grounds? Oikos. 1978;30:405–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Richardson JW. Northeastward reverse migration of birds over Nova Scotia, Canada, in autumn. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1982;10:193–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Taylor PD, Mackenzie SA, Thurber BG, Calvert AM, Mill AM, McGuire LP et al. Landscape movements of migratory birds and bats reveal an expanded scale of stopover. PLoS ONE. 2011; doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0027054.

  17. Akesson S, Walinder G, Karlsson L, Ehnbom S. Nocturnal migratory flight initiation in reed warblers Acrocephalus scirpaceus: effect of wind on orientation and timing of migration. J Avian Biol. 2002;33:349–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Van Doren BM, Sheldon D, Geevarghese J, Hochachka WM, Farnsworth A. Autumn morning flights of migrant songbirds in the northeastern United States are linked to nocturnal migration and winds aloft. Auk. 2014;132:105–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Able KP, Able MA. The flexible migratory orientation system of the Savannah Sparrow (Passerculus sandwichensis). J Exp Biol. 1996;199(Pt 1):3–8.

  20. Helbig AJ. Genetic basis, mode of inheritance and evolutionary changes of migratory directions in palearctic warblers (Aves: sylviidae). J Exp Biol. 1996;199(Pt 1) :49–55.

  21. Sutherland WJ. Evidence for flexibility and constraint in migration systems. J Avian Biol. 1998; doi:10.2307/3677163.

  22. Thorup K, Bisson I-A, Bowlin MS, Holland RA, Wingfield JC, Ramenofsky M et al. Evidence for a navigational map stretching across the continental U.S. in a migratory songbird. PNAS. 2007; doi:10.1073/pnas.0704734104.

  23. Schaub M, Jenni L, Bairlein F. Fuel stores, fuel accumulation, and the decision to depart from a migration stopover site. Behav Ecol. 2008; doi:10.1093/beheco/arn023.

  24. Åkesson S, Karlsson L, Walinder G, Alerstam T. Bimodal orientation and the occurrence of temporary reverse bird migration during autumn in south Scandinavia. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1996; doi:10.1007/s002650050245.

  25. Woodworth BK, Francis CM, Taylor PD. Inland flights of young red-eyed vireos Vireo olivaceus in relation to survival and habitat in a coastal stopover landscape. J Avian Biol. 2014; doi:10.1111/jav.00276.

  26. Hake M, Kjellén N, Alerstam T. Age-dependent migration strategy in honey buzzards Pernis apivorus tracked by satellite. Oikos. 2003;103:385–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Bruderer B, Liechti F. Flight behavior of nocturnally migrating birds in coastal areas – crossing or coasting. J Avian Biol. 1998;29:499–507.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Stobo WT, McLaren IA. The Ipswich Sparrow. Vol. 27. Los Angeles, CA: Nova Scotian Institute of Science; 1975.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Dale CA, Leonard ML. Reproductive consequences of migration decisions by Ipswich Sparrows (Passerculus sandwichensis princeps). Can J Zool. 2011; doi:10.1139/Z10-098.

  30. Cox WA, Thompson FR, Cox AS, Faaborg J. Post-fledging survival in passerine birds and the value of post-fledging studies to conservation. J Wild Man. 2014;78:183–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Rappole JH, Tipton AR. New harness design for attachment of radio transmitters to small passerines. J Field Ornithol. 1991;62:335–7.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Barron DG, Brawn JD, Weatherhead PJ. Meta-analysis of transmitter effects on avian behavior and ecology. Methods Ecol Evol. 2010; doi:10.1111/j.2041-210X.2010.00013.x.

  33. Mills AM, Thurber BG, Mackenzie SA, Taylor PD. Passerines use nocturnal flight for landscape-scale movements during migration stopover. Condor. 2011;113:597–706.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Hijmans RJ. Geosphere: Spherical Trigonometry R package version 1.3-11. 2014 [http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/geosphere].

  35. Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B, Walker S. lme4: linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4 R package version 1.1. 2014 [http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lme4].

  36. Agostinelli C, Lund U. circular: Circular Statistics R package version 0.4-7. 2013 [http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/circular].

  37. Lund U, Agostinelli C. The CircStats package R Package Version 0.2-4. 2009 [http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/CircStats/index.html].

  38. Handel CM, Gill RE. Wayward youth: trans-Beringian movement and differential southward migration by juvenile sharp-tailed sandpipers. Arctic. 2010; doi:10.14430/arctic1492.

  39. Agostini N, Panuccio M, Lucia G, Luizzi C, Amato P. Evidence for age-dependent migration strategies in the Short-toes Eagle. Br Birds. 2009;102:506–508.

    Google Scholar 

  40. Brown JM, Taylor PD. Adult and hatch-year blackpoll warblers exhibit radically different regional movements during post-fledging dispersal. Biol Letters. 2015; doi:10.1098/rsbl.2015.0593.

  41. Ydenberg RC, Butler RW, Lank DB. Effects of predator landscapes on the evolutionary ecology of routing, timing and molt by long-distance migrants. J Avian Biol. 2007; doi:10.1111/j.2007.0908-8857.04202.x.

  42. Bauer S, Ens BJ, Klaassen M. Many routes lead to Rome: potential causes for the multi-route migration system of Red Knots, Calidris canutus islandica. Ecology. 2010;91:1822–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. La Sorte FA, Hochachka WM, Farnsworth A, Sheldon D, Fink D, Geevarghese J et al. Migration timing and its determinants for nocturnal migratory birds during autumn migration. J Anim Ecol. 2015; doi:10.1111/1365-2656.12376.

  44. Cimprich DA, Woodrey MS, Moore FR. Passerine migrants respond to variation in predation risk during stopover. Anim Behav. 2005;69:1173–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Gill RE, Tibbitts TL, Douglas DC, Handel CM, Mulcahy DM, Gottschalck JC et al. Extreme endurance flights by landbirds crossing the Pacific Ocean: ecological corridor rather than barrier? Proc R Soc B. 2009; doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.1142.

  46. Mellone U, Limiñana R, Mallia E, Urios V. Extremely detoured migration in an inexperienced bird: interplay of transport costs and social interactions. J Avian Biol. 2011; doi:10.1111/j.1600-048X.2011.05454.x.

  47. Salewski V, Almasi B, Heuman A, Thoma M, Schlageter A. Agonistic behavior of Palearctic passerine migrants at a stopover site suggests interference competition. J Afri Ornith. 2007;78:349–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Mills AM, Thurber BG, Mackenzie SA, Taylor PD. Passerines use nocturnal flights for landscape-scale movements during migration stopover. Condor. 2011; doi:10.1525/cond.2011.100186.

  49. Woodrey MS. Age-dependent aspects of stopover biology of passerine migrants. Stud Avian Biol. 2000;20:43–52.

    Google Scholar 

  50. Chernetsov N. Habitat selection by nocturnal passerine migrants en route: mechanisms and results. J Ornith. 2006; doi:10.1007/s10336-006-0064-6.

  51. Gardali T, Barton DC, White JD, Geupel GR. Juvenile and adult survival of Swainson’s Thrush (Catharus ustulatus) in coastal California: annual estimates using capture-recapture analyses. Auk. 2003;120:1188–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Stutchbury BJM, Gow EA, Done T, MacPherson M, Fox W, Afanasyev V. Effects of post-breeding moult and energetic condition on timing of songbird migration into the tropics. Proc R Soc B. 2011;278:131–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank I. Pollet, B. Woodworth, J. Stephens, C. Craig, H. Lightfoot, S. Wong, M. Thillet, G. Thurber, M. O’Brien, and L. Adams for assistance with field work and logistics. Bird Studies Canada provided extensive field and logistic support in association with the Motus Telemetry Array. Other Motus particpants in the Gulf of Maine (UMASS, UMAINE, USFWS) provided data from their receivers stations that were crucial to the project. We would also like to thank the reviewers for their insightful and constructive comments which led to a much improved manuscript.

Funding

This work was funded by Encana Corporation’s Deep Panuke Education & Training and Research & Development Fund, Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) Collaborative Research and Development grant, and the Canadian Wildlife Federation. In kind support was provided by Environment Canada, Parks Canada, Department of Fisheries and Oceans, and ExxonMobil.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Zoe J. Crysler.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

PDT and RAR conceived and designed the study. ZJC and PDT analyzed the data and wrote the manuscript. RAR provided comments and reviewed the manuscript. ZJC and RAR conducted field work. PDT and RAR provided the funding for this research. All authors have read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Additional file

Additional file 1:

Receiver station locations and antenna orientations. Start and Stop times refer to the period each receiver was active. Some changes in antenna orientation occurred (eg. CANS1 & CANS2) however sites remained active during orientation changes and therefore were unlikely to impact detections during this study. Receivers were maintained and checked for operation on a cycle of weeks or months depending on site accessibility. Evaluation of raw data showed no major receiver malfunctions except at 3 locations, all of which should not have affected data: CANS1 was non-operational for 3 days (Nov 10–13) however CANS2 was within 1 km and was operational during this time; KEJI was non-operational for 8 days (between Nov 8–19) however only one individual was detected for the first time after Nov 8th and it was first detected 150 km NE at CONRD, thus this likely did not affect overwater flight information from Sable Island to the mainland; STAGE had frequent blackouts however LOT1 was within 5 km. (XLS 37 kb)

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Crysler, Z.J., Ronconi, R.A. & Taylor, P.D. Differential fall migratory routes of adult and juvenile Ipswich Sparrows (Passerculus sandwichensis princeps). Mov Ecol 4, 3 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40462-016-0067-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40462-016-0067-8

Keywords